Abstract
Positron emission tomography has, for 30 years, been used in numerous case-control studies searching for hypothesized differences in the density of neuroreceptor or transporter proteins in psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and depression. In most cases, the results have not been conclusive. One reason could be the sizeable interindividual variability in biochemical markers, which in twin studies have shown to emanate from both environmental and genetic factors, leading to low statistical power for the detection of group effects. On the other hand, the same interindividual variability has served as an opportunity for correlative studies on the biological underpinning of behaviour. Using this approach, a series of studies has linked markers for the dopamine and serotonin system to personality traits associated with psychiatric conditions. Based on increasing evidence for the view that many psychopathological states represent extremes of a continuum rather than distinct categories, this research strategy may lead to new biological insights about the vulnerability to and pathophysiology of major psychiatric disorders.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Diverse perspectives on diversity: multi-disciplinary approaches to taxonomies of individual differences'.
Keywords: positron emission tomography, dopamine, serotonin, neuroreceptors, psychiatric disorders, personality traits
1. Background
In early 1980s, methods were developed for quantification of brain neuroreceptors in humans in vivo using positron emission tomography (PET). The efforts were to a significant degree driven by the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia, postulating that the pathophysiology of this disorder is related to elevated dopaminergic transmission. The assumption in initial PET studies was that this overactivity was related to increased density of the D2-dopamine receptor (D2R) subtype. This somewhat simplistic model was justified by replicated findings of elevated D2R in brains of patients with schizophrenia post mortem [1], and experimental studies demonstrating that neuroleptic drugs are D2R antagonists. Beyond the primary aim of demonstrating significant differences between healthy subjects and young neuroleptic naive patients with schizophrenia, there was also a hope for a large separation between the groups. If a distinct separation was present, then D2R density could serve as a clinically useful and much sought for diagnostic marker for schizophrenia.
Over the years, numerous PET studies have been carried out in both neuroleptic naive and drug-treated patient samples. The overall view, supported by meta-analyses, is that the findings of elevated D2R post mortem cannot be replicated in vivo [2,3].
However, the concept of searching for a single biochemical abnormality in patients has been extended to other potential biomarkers of the dopamine system, as well as to other neurotransmission systems and disorders. For instance, following the development of radioligands for PET-imaging of the serotonin (5-HT) system, and supported by pharmacological evidence, many studies have been conducted in depression and anxiety. The results are not conclusive. Some reports suggest decreases in serotonin transporter levels [4], whereas studies on the 5-HT1A receptor have shown mixed results [5,6] and only two small studies have reported changes in 5-HT1B receptor binding [7,8].
In most such studies, the primary outcome used to compare patients and control subjects is the binding potential (BPnd), which represents the ratio between receptor density (Bmax) and affinity (Kd) [9,10]. BPnd is commonly used as an index for density because Kd is assumed to be constant, albeit influenced by the endogenous neurotransmitter concentration. In the following text, we use the concept ‘density’ when we refer to BPnd-values.
The aim of the present commentary is to review some of the studies on associations between imaging markers and personality traits, and to discuss if they can represent a valuable approach to understanding the underpinnings of mental illness.
2. Interindividual variability in neuroreceptor density
The initial discovery of neuroreceptors was a result of experimental pharmacological studies using inbred animal strains, where interindividual variability in receptor density is not a major concern. When translating this field of experimental research to humans, a different picture emerged. For instance, in a study of more than 200 human brains post mortem, a nearly fourfold range was reported for the striatal D2R density [11]. This finding of a large interindividual variability was later replicated in vivo using PET [12]. In this study, individual D2R density (Bmax) and affinity (Kd) were calculated from a saturation analysis based on five PET-measurements in each of 10 males and 10 females. There was a 2.5-fold range in D2R density. Similar ranges of variability have been reported also for other neuroreceptors [13–19].
PET is costly and the recruitment of subjects is demanding, in particular if drug-free or drug-naive patients are required. By consequence, patient samples have been small in a majority of clinical studies, typically fewer than 20. When combined with the sizeable interindividual variability, this has led to low statistical power for the detection of group effects. This is especially the case for psychiatric disorders devoid of known histopathology, where group differences are expected to be small. For instance, arguably the most robust observation of a neurotransmission marker in psychiatric patients is an elevation of the presynaptic marker [18F]FDOPA in schizophrenia patients, showing an effect size of 0.79 (Cohen's D) [2]. In comparison, radioligand binding to the dopamine transporter (DAT) in Parkinson's Disease, which is characterized by a major loss of cells, is reduced by about 50% already at clinical onset, with effect sizes up to Cohen's D = 3.8 for striatal regions later in the disease stage [20]. Hence, whereas DAT-imaging using PET or SPECT (single photon emission computed tomography) is a long-established diagnostic tool in Parkinson's Disease [21], thus far there are no such PET markers suitable for such clinical use in psychiatric populations.
3. Sources of variability
Despite the high interest in the serotonin and dopamine neurotransmission systems in psychiatry research, little is known about the regulation of receptor and transporter density levels. Considering the high heritability of major psychiatric disorders, it is of fundamental interest to understand if their densities in adult life are genetically determined or influenced by the environment. This lack of knowledge limits the interpretation of changes in protein availability reported in psychiatric patients. In a recent attempt to elucidate this issue, we used PET in a twin design to estimate the relative contribution of genetic and environmental factors, respectively, on dopaminergic and serotonergic markers in the living human brain [14]. Heritability, shared environmental effects and individual-specific non-shared effects were estimated for 5-HT1A receptor availability in serotonergic projection areas and for D2R in striatum. We found a major contribution of genetic factors (heritability 0.67; shared environment effect 0.00; non-shared environment effect 0.33) on individual variability in striatal D2R binding and a major contribution of environmental factors (heritability 0.17–0.22; pair-wise shared environment effect 0.70–0.75; unique individual effect 0.08) on neocortical 5-HT1A receptor binding. Interestingly, the heritability for D2R was in a similar range, as was previously reported for the presynaptic marker [18F]FDOPA [22]. These results confirm that both genetic and environmental factors should be taken into account in disease models of psychiatric disorders that are based on aberrations in the brain neurotransmission systems.
4. Correlative studies: personality traits
Though interindividual variability is a problem for comparisons aiming for large separations of groups, it has shown to serve as an opportunity for correlative studies on the biological underpinning of behavioural markers in healthy control subjects. An area of specific interest has been stable patterns of behaviour, cognition and emotion conceptualized as personality traits, typically measured by self-assessment questionnaires. These traits are generally characterized by a substantial heritability, both for scales traditionally referred to as measuring ‘temperament’ and for ‘personality’ scales [23,24] and have shown to be important predictors for psychiatric disorders [25,26]. Consequently, markers of brain neurotransmission may serve as useful means of tracking down gene–protein-behavioural pathways towards psychiatric disease. This approach is in line with the view that psychiatric disorders may represent extremes on a continuum rather than being qualitatively different from normal behaviour—even for psychotic disorders [27,28]. Indeed, moving towards a dimensional approach of diagnosis rather than the existing prototypic classification was an early ambition for the revised edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5 [29]). However, this development has been hampered by a lack of biological validation of the proposed models.
In an early study on personality traits and neuroreceptor density, The Karolinska Scales of Personality (KSP) was administered to 18 of the 20 healthy subjects in the D2R study mentioned earlier [12]. KSP is a self-rating instrument, and measures 15 different personality traits that were developed to be sensitive for vulnerability of mental illness [30]. There was a significant correlation between striatal D2R density and detachment, a measure of social avoidance and withdrawal [31]. This finding was shortly thereafter replicated by an independent centre [32] and initiated a series of similar molecular imaging studies summarized in tables 1 and 2.
Table 1.
personality trait | marker | tracer | n | region | r | publication |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
detachment (KSP) | D2 | RAC | 24; 18 | striatum | − | Farde et al. [31]; Breier et al. [32] |
DAT | CFT | 18 | striatum | − | Laakso et al. [53] | |
attachment (TCI) | D2/D3 | PHNO | 32 | VST | − | Caravaggio et al. [38] |
novelty seeking (TCI) | D2 | FLB | 24 | right insula | − | Suhara et al. [54] |
novelty seeking (TPQ) | D2 | FP | 34 | midbrain | − | Zald et al. [55] |
sensation seeking (ZS) | D2 | RAC | 18 | striatum | nl | Gjedde et al. [56] |
venturesomeness (I7) | D2 | FP | 18 | TC, thalamus | + | Bernow et al. [57] |
impulsivity (NEO-PI-R) | DA release | RAC | 40 | right VST | − | Oswald et al. [58] |
impulsivity (BIS) | D2 | FP | 32 | midbrain | − | Buckholtz et al. [59] |
impulsivity (BIS) | DA release | striatum | + | |||
depression (NEO-PI-R) | D2 | RAC | 18 | striatum | + | Kestler et al. [60] |
harm avoidance (TCI) | D2 | RAC | 21 | dorsal striatum | − | Kim et al. [61] |
anxiety, irritability (KSP) | DOPA | FDOPA | 33 | striatum | − | Laakso et al. [62] |
lie scale (MPI) | D2 | IB | 42 | striatum | − | Huang et al. [18] |
lie scale (MPI, EPQ-R) | D2 | RAC | 28; 13; 23 | striatum | − | Reeves et al. [34], Egerton et al. [35] |
lie scale (EPQ-R) | DOPA | FDOPA | 46 | striatum | n.s. | Stokes et al. [63] |
social desirability (SSP) | D2 | RAC; FLB | 16 | striatum, MTL | − | Cervenka et al. [33] |
social desirability (SSP) | D1 | SCH | 23 | striatum/amyg, FC | + | Plavén-Sigray et al. [64] |
+ physical trait aggression (SSP) | − | |||||
socialization (KSP) | D2 | RAC | 30 | VST | + | Caravaggio et al. [65] |
socioeconomic status (HS) | D2 | RAC | 42 | striatum | + | Wiers et al. [36] |
social status (BSMSS) | D2 | RAC | 14 | striatum | + | Martinez et al. [37] |
social status (BSMSS) | D2/D3 | PHNO | 16 | SN/VTA | − | Matuskey et al. [38] |
Table 2.
personality trait | marker | tracer | n | region | r | publication |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
neuroticism (NEO-PI-R) | 5-HTT | DASB | 31 | thalamus | + | Takano et al. [66] |
harm avoidance (TCI) | 5-HTT | DASB | 19 | n.s. | Reimold et al. [67] | |
harm avoidance (TCI) | 5-HTT | MADAM | 22 | n.s. | Tuominen et al. [48] | |
neuroticism (NEO-PI-R) | 5-HT1A | WAY | 19 | DLPFC, PC, OC, ACC | − | Tauscher et al. [19] |
harm avoidance (TPQ); neuroticism (EPQ); state anxiety (STAI) | 5-HT1A | WAY | 49; 44; 22 | n.s. | Rabiner et al. [13] | |
harm avoidance (TCI) | 5-HT1A | WAY | 15 | n.s. | Borg et al. [68] | |
neuroticism (KSP) | 5-HT1A | WAY | 34 | DLPFC; STG, HC | − | Hirvonen et al. [47] |
harm avoidance (TPQ) | 5-HT2A | FESP | 11 | FC, PC | − | Moresco et al. [69] |
neuroticism (NEO-PI-R) | 5-HT2A | ALT | 83 | frontolimbic | + | Frokjaer et al. [70] |
harm avoidance (TCI) | 5-HT2A | ALT | 21 | n.s. | Soloff et al. [71] | |
harm avoidance (TCI) | 5-HT2A | ALT | 27 | HC | + | Soloff et al. [72] |
dysfunctional attitudes | 5-HTT | DASB | 20 | n.s. | Meyer et al. [73] | |
life-time aggression (BGLHA) | 5-HT1A | WAY | 25 | FC, raphe | − | Parsey et al. [74] |
questionnaire for measuring factors of aggression | 5-HT1A | WAY | 36 | ACC | + | Witte et al. [17] |
life-time aggression (BGLHA) | 5-HT2A | ALT | 21 | n.s. | Soloff et al. [71] | |
trait aggression (BPAQ); trait impulsivity (BIS-11); angry hostility (NEO-PI-R) | 5-HT2A | ALT | 94 | n.s. | da Cunha-Bang et al. [75] | |
trait aggression (BPAQ); trait impulsivity (BIS-11) | 5-HT4 | SB | 47 males (n.s. in females) | whole brain | + | da Cunha-Bang et al. [76] |
openness (NEO-PI-R) | 5-HTT | DASB | 50 | midbrain, putamen, thalamus | − | Kalbitzer et al. [77] |
self-transcendence/spiritual acceptance (TCI) | 5-HTT | DASB | 16 | raphe | − | Kim et al. [78] |
self-transcendence/spiritual acceptance (TCI) | 5-HT1A | WAY | 15 | neocortex, HC, raphe | − | Borg et al. [68] |
self-transcendence/spiritual acceptance (TCI) | 5-HT1A | WAY | 20 | n.s. | Karlsson et al. [79] | |
reward dependence (TCI) | 5-HT2A | SET | 24 | ACC, OFC | − | Gerretsen et al. [80] |
reward dependence (TCI) | 5-HT2A | ALT | 21 | n.s. | Soloff et al. [71] | |
reward dependence (TCI) | 5-HT2A | ALT | 27 | rPRG | + | Soloff et al. [72] |
straightforwardness, trust; low tolerance of unfairness (NEO-PI-R; UG) | 5-HTT | DASB | 20 | midbrain | − | Takahashi et al. [81] |
From this literature, it is clear that several brain proteins may serve as correlates to personality traits. Importantly, a reported and replicated correlation between a single receptor or transport protein and a certain personality trait does not imply that the protein per se has a causal role in neuronal mechanisms related to the trait or that the density may serve as a biochemical marker for the trait. However, by indicating that a certain neurotransmission system is part of the biological underpinning of a trait, these findings may lead to biological insights and hypotheses about the vulnerability, genesis and progress of psychiatric disorders. One advantage of this research strategy is to circumvent the influence of unspecific factors, such as stress and insomnia, that often characterize the transition from a high risk or vulnerable state into a full-blown psychiatric condition.
As can be seen from table 1, initial findings of a relationship between D2R and traits related to social behaviour have been corroborated in multiple studies when using antagonist radioligands. Specifically, a negative correlation has been reported between D2R and the social desirability scale of the Swedish University Scales of Personality (SSP) [33] and Lie scale of the Maudsley Personality Inventory (MPI) [18,34,35], a line of research that has also been extended to include measures of social status [36,37]. Interestingly, recent studies using the agonist radioligand [11C]PHNO have shown patterns of the opposite direction [38,39]. It may be speculated that this is an effect of the increased sensitivity for endogenous dopamine (DA) levels, suggesting that previous studies in part may be influenced by synaptic DA. Alternatively, it may be a consequence of the relatively higher D3 dopamine receptor affinity for [11C]PHNO [40]. Taken together, these findings suggest an intricate relationship between DA function and social behaviour, and may serve as a starting point for investigations in relevant patient groups [41,42].
With regard to psychosis-related traits, there are to date no reports on associations with D2R, mirroring findings in patients. By contrast, striatal amphetamine-induced DA release has shown to be associated with schizotypal personality traits [43], which is in line with reports of increases in DA release and the presynaptic marker [18F]DOPA in schizophrenia patients and individuals at high risk for the disorder [2,44–46].
For the serotonin system, focus has been mainly on traits related to anxiety and mood disorders, but, the results have been less clear. A strong negative correlation has been shown between 5-HT1A receptor and neuroticism, a trait associated with vulnerability for anxiety and depression [47], whereas no associations were found for the serotonin transporter (5HTT) [48]. Following our observations of a strong contribution of environmental factors for the 5-HT1A receptor, we recently studied the effect of seasonal and diurnal variation on the serotonin system by combining healthy control subjects from several small individual studies [49]. In this sample including 96 PET-measurements, we observed decreases in midbrain 5HTT during the day, and higher 5-HT1A receptor availability on days with longer daylight. The observations suggest a link between the serotonin system and observations of disruptions in chronobiology in both seasonal and non-seasonal affective disorders [50–52].
Importantly, while many associations between behavioural traits and neurotransmitter receptor density have been replicated by independent centres, this is not the case for all findings depicted in tables 1 and 2. Low reliability of measurements, small sample sizes and the failure to publish inconclusive replication attempts could potentially lead to a number of false positives being left uncontested in the literature. Possible remedies for this are open sharing of data and code [82], as well as pre-registration of new trials [83]. Another caveat that needs to be considered when interpreting association between biochemical outcomes and personality traits is that the samples might not always be perfect representations of the general population, such that the personality of individuals volunteering to research might differ on certain traits [84].
The examples listed in tables 1 and 2, involving the two major neurotransmitter systems implicated in treatment of psychiatric patients, show that research into biological correlates of stable behavioural phenotypes may be a way forward to gain insights regarding disease mechanisms of psychiatric disorders. This approach is also in line with the shift from categorical to a more dimensional conceptualization of psychiatric diagnoses. However, small effect sizes are likely to be continually expected in the field of PET and psychiatry, and with more studies moving from being exploratory to being confirmatory, increased statistical power will be required. For this reason, we believe an increased focus on multicentre collaboration is necessary.
Another way forward is to go beyond mere association between neuroreceptor density and self-reported personality dimensions, and to examine the neurobiology of underlying behavioural phenotypes. This can be done by measuring behaviour in an experimental setting, and as such, pinpointing the constituents that are driving the relationships reported in tables 1 and 2. Current attempts in this direction include studies showing associations between [18F]FDOPA uptake and paradigms of prediction error coding [85] and salience attribution [86], which are both of interest in relation to cognitive models of schizophrenia [87]. With regard to traits tapping social behaviour, we suggest that future studies should examine the relationship between dopamine receptor availability and social trust and status, as measured using paradigms from behavioural economics and social psychology [88]. Observation of such less complex, observable traits both within a patient group and across different diagnostic groups, can aid discovery of behavioural and biological diagnostic markers as well as markers of vulnerability. It may also enhance translational science between species, and therefore facilitate more precise studies of molecular mechanisms in drug development.
In summary, we think that investigations of the biological underpinnings of personality traits and their constituents using molecular imaging techniques hold several advantages, and may lead to biological insights regarding the genesis, progress and, ultimately, treatment of psychiatric disorder.
Data accessibility
This article has no additional data.
Authors' contributions
L.F. outlined the main scope of the commentary. P.P.-S., J.B. and S.C. revised the text and contributed additional text paragraphs and suitable references. All authors contributed to the final integrated version and gave final approval for publication.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests related to the subject matter.
Funding
This study was supported by grant nos 2015-02398 to L.F. and 523-2014-3467 to S.C. from the Swedish Research Council.
References
- 1.Seeman P, Ulpian C, Bergeron C, Riederer P, Jellinger K, Gabriel E, Reynolds G, Tourtellotte W. 1984. Bimodal distribution of dopamine receptor densities in brains of schizophrenics. Science 225, 728–732. ( 10.1126/science.6147018) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Howes OD, Kambeitz J, Kim E, Stahl D, Slifstein M, Abi-Dargham A, Kapur S. 2012. The nature of dopamine dysfunction in schizophrenia and what this means for treatment. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 776–786. ( 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.169) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kambeitz J, Abi-Dargham A, Kapur S, Howes OD. 2014. Alterations in cortical and extrastriatal subcortical dopamine function in schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis of imaging studies. Br. J. Psychiatry 204, 420–429. ( 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.132308) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Spies M, Knudsen GM, Lanzenberger R, Kasper S. 2015. The serotonin transporter in psychiatric disorders: insights from PET imaging. Lancet Psychiatry 2, 743–755. ( 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00232-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hirvonen J, Karlsson H, Kajander J, Lepola A, Markkula J, Rasi-Hakala H, Någren K, Salminen JK, Hietala J. 2008. Decreased brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor availability in medication-naive patients with major depressive disorder: an in-vivo imaging study using PET and [carbonyl-11C]WAY-100635. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 11, 465–476. ( 10.1017/S1461145707008140) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hesselgrave N, Parsey RV. 2013. Imaging the serotonin 1A receptor using [11C]WAY100635 in healthy controls and major depression . Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 1–6. ( 10.1098/rstb.2012.0004) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Tiger M, Rück C, Forsberg A, Varrone A, Lindefors N, Halldin C, Farde L, Lundberg J. 2014. Reduced 5-HT(1B) receptor binding in the dorsal brain stem after cognitive behavioural therapy of major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 223, 164–170. ( 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.05.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Murrough JW, Henry S, Hu J, Gallezot J-D, Planeta-Wilson B, Neumaier JF, Neumeister A. 2011. Reduced ventral striatal/ventral pallidal serotonin1B receptor binding potential in major depressive disorder. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 213, 547–553. ( 10.1007/s00213-010-1881-0) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mintun MA, Raichle ME, Kilbourn MR, Wooten GF, Welch MJ. 1984. A quantitative model for the in vivo assessment of drug binding sites with positron emission tomography. Ann. Neurol. 15, 217–227. ( 10.1002/ana.410150302) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Innis RB, et al. 2007. Consensus nomenclature for in vivo imaging of reversibly binding radioligands. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 27, 1533–1539. ( 10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600493) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Seeman P. 1987. The absolute density of neurotransmitter receptors in the brain. Example for dopamine receptors. J. Pharmacol. Methods 17, 347–360. ( 10.1016/0160-5402(87)90048-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Farde L, Hall H, Pauli S, Halldin C. 1995. Variability in D2-dopamine receptor density and affinity: a PET study with [11C]raclopride in man. Synapse 20, 200–208. ( 10.1002/syn.890200303) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rabiner EA, et al. 2002. A database of [11C]WAY-100635 binding to 5-HT1A receptors in normal male volunteers: normative data and relationship to methodological, demographic, physiological, and behavioral variables. Neuroimage 15, 620–632. ( 10.1006/nimg.2001.0984) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Borg J, et al. 2016. Contribution of non-genetic factors to dopamine and serotonin receptor availability in the adult human brain. Mol. Psychiatry 21, 1077–1084. ( 10.1038/mp.2015.147) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hirvonen J, Aalto S, Hagelberg N, Maksimow A, Ingman K, Oikonen V, Virkkala J, Någren K, Scheinin H. 2009. Measurement of central μ-opioid receptor binding in vivo with PET and [11C]carfentanil: a test–retest study in healthy subjects. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 36, 275–286. ( 10.1007/s00259-008-0935-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Karlsson P, Farde L, Halldin C, Sedvall G. 2002. PET study of D1 dopamine receptor binding in neuroleptic-naive patients with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 761–767. ( 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.761) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Witte AV, et al. 2009. Aggression is related to frontal serotonin-1A receptor distribution as revealed by PET in healthy subjects. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2558–2570. ( 10.1002/hbm.20687) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Huang CL, Yang YK, Chu CL, Lee IH, Yeh TL, Chen PS, Chiu NT. 2006. The association between the Lie scale of the Maudsley personality inventory and striatal dopamine D2/D3 receptor availability of healthy Chinese community subjects. Eur. Psychiatry 21, 62–65. ( 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2005.05.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Tauscher J, Bagby RM, Javanmard M, Christensen BK, Kasper S, Kapur S. 2001. Inverse relationship between serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding and anxiety: a [11C] WAY-100635 PET investigation in healthy volunteers. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1326–1328. ( 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1326) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Fazio P, et al. 2015. Quantitative Analysis of 18F-(E)-N-(3-iodoprop-2-Enyl)-2β-carbofluoroethoxy-3β-(4′-methyl-phenyl) nortropane binding to the dopamine transporter in Parkinson Disease. J. Nucl. Med. 56, 714–720. ( 10.2967/jnumed.114.152421) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Marek KL, et al. 1996. [123I] beta-CIT/SPECT imaging demonstrates bilateral loss of dopamine transporters in hemi-Parkinson's disease. Neurology 46, 231–237. ( 10.1212/WNL.46.1.231) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Stokes PRA, Shotbolt P, Mehta MA, Turkheimer E, Benecke A, Copeland C, Turkheimer FE, Lingford-Hughes AR, Howes OD. 2013. Nature or nurture? Determining the heritability of human striatal dopamine function: an [18F]-DOPA PET Study. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 485–491. ( 10.1038/npp.2012.207) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bouchard T, Lykken D, McGue M, Segal N, Tellegen A. 1990. Sources of human psychological differences: the Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science 250, 223–228. ( 10.1126/science.2218526) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Vukasović T, Bratko D. 2015. Heritability of personality: a meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies. Psychol. Bull. 141, 769–785. ( 10.1037/bul0000017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bienvenu OJ, Hettema JM, Neale MC, Prescott CA, Kendler KS. 2007. Low extraversion and high neuroticism as indices of genetic and environmental risk for social phobia, agoraphobia, and animal phobia. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 1714–1721. ( 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06101667) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kendler KS, Kuhn J, Prescott CA. 2004. The interrelationship of neuroticism, sex, and stressful life events in the prediction of episodes of major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 631–636. ( 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.4.631) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. 2009. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness–persistence–impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol. Med. 39, 179 ( 10.1017/S0033291708003814) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Freeman D. 2006. Delusions in the nonclinical population. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 8, 191–204. ( 10.1007/s11920-006-0023-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schalling D, Edman G. 1993. The Karolinska scales of personality (KSP) manual: an inventory for assessing temperament dimensions associated with vulnerability for psychosocial deviance. Stockholm, Sweden: Department of Psychiatry, Karolinska Institutet. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Farde L, Gustavsson JP, Jönsson E. 1997. D2 dopamine receptors and personality traits. Nature 385, 590 ( 10.1038/385590a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Breier A, Kestler L, Adler C, Elman I, Wiesenfeld N, Malhotra A, Pickar D. 1998. Dopamine D2 receptor density and personal detachment in healthy subjects. Am. J. Psychiatry 155, 1440–1442. ( 10.1176/ajp.155.10.1440) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cervenka S, Gustavsson P, Halldin C, Farde L. 2010. Association between striatal and extrastriatal dopamine D2-receptor binding and social desirability. Neuroimage 50, 323–328. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Reeves SJ, Mehta MA, Montgomery AJ, Amiras D, Egerton A, Howard RJ, Grasby PM. 2007. Striatal dopamine (D2) receptor availability predicts socially desirable responding. Neuroimage 34, 1782–1789. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.042) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Egerton A, Rees E, Bose SK, Lappin JM, Stokes PRA, Turkheimer FE, Reeves SJ. 2010. Truth, lies or self-deception? Striatal D(2/3) receptor availability predicts individual differences in social conformity. Neuroimage 53, 777–781. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.031) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wiers CE, Shokri-kojori E, Cabrera E, Cunningham S, Wong C, Tomasi D, Wang GJ, Volkow ND. 2016. Socioeconomic status is associated with striatal dopamine D2/D3 receptors in healthy volunteers but not in cocaine abusers. Neurosci. Lett. 617, 27–31. ( 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.01.056) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Martinez D, Orlowska D, Narendran R, Slifstein M, Liu F, Kumar D, Broft A, Van Heertum R, Kleber HD. 2009. Dopamine type 2/3 receptor availability in the striatum and social status in human volunteers. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 275–278. ( 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.037) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Caravaggio F, Chung JK, Gerretsen P, Fervaha G, Nakajima S, Plitman E, Iwata Y, Wilson A, Graff-Guerrero A. 2017. Exploring the relationship between social attachment and dopamine D2/3 receptor availability in the brains of healthy humans using [11C]-(+)-PHNO. Soc. Neurosci. 12, 163–173. ( 10.1080/17470919.2016.1152997) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Matuskey D, et al. 2015. A preliminary study of dopamine D2/3 receptor availability and social status in healthy and cocaine dependent humans imaged with [11C](+)PHNO. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 154, 167–173. ( 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.06.039) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Tziortzi AC, Searle GE, Tzimopoulou S, Salinas C, Beaver JD, Jenkinson M, Laruelle M, Rabiner EA, Gunn RN. 2011. Imaging dopamine receptors in humans with [11C]-(+)-PHNO: dissection of D3 signal and anatomy. Neuroimage 54, 264–277. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.044) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Cervenka S, Hedman E, Ikoma Y, Djurfeldt D, Rück C, Halldin C, Lindefors N. 2012. Changes in dopamine D2-receptor binding are associated to symptom reduction after psychotherapy in social anxiety disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 2, e120 ( 10.1038/tp.2012.40) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Plavén-Sigray P, et al. 2017. Extrastriatal dopamine D2-receptor availability in social anxiety disorder. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 27, 462–469. ( 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.03.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Woodward ND, Cowan RL, Park S, Ansari MS, Baldwin RM, Li R, Doop M, Kessler RM, Zald DH. 2011. Correlation of individual differences in schizotypal personality traits with amphetamine-induced dopamine release in striatal and extrastriatal brain regions. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 418–426. ( 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10020165) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Breier A, et al. 1997. Schizophrenia is associated with elevated amphetamine-induced synaptic dopamine concentrations: evidence from a novel positron emission tomography method. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 94, 2569–2574. ( 10.1073/pnas.94.6.2569) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Laruelle M, et al. 1996. Single photon emission computerized tomography imaging of amphetamine-induced dopamine release in drug-free schizophrenic subjects. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 93, 9235–9240. ( 10.1073/pnas.93.17.9235) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Howes OD, et al. 2009. Elevated striatal dopamine function linked to prodromal signs of schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 13–20. ( 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.514) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hirvonen J, Tuominen L, Någren K, Hietala J. 2015. Neuroticism and serotonin 5-HT1A receptors in healthy subjects. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 234, 1–6. ( 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.04.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tuominen L, et al. 2013. Temperament, character and serotonin activity in the human brain: a positron emission tomography study based on a general population cohort. Psychol. Med. 43, 881–894. ( 10.1017/S003329171200164X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Matheson GJ, Schain M, Almeida R, Lundberg J, Cselényi Z, Borg J, Varrone A, Farde L, Cervenka S. 2015. Diurnal and seasonal variation of the brain serotonin system in healthy male subjects. Neuroimage 112, 225–231. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rosenthal NE, Sack DA, Gillin JC, Lewy AJ, Goodwin FK, Davenport Y, Mueller PS, Newsome DA, Wehr TA. 1984. Seasonal affective disorder. A description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 41, 72–80. ( 10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790120076010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Wirz-Justice A. 2006. Biological rhythm disturbances in mood disorders. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 21(Suppl. 1), S11–S15. ( 10.1097/01.yic.0000195660.37267.cf) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Li JZ, Bunney BG, Meng F, Hagenauer MH, Walsh DM, Vawter MP. 2013. Circadian patterns of gene expression in the human brain and disruption in major depressive disorder. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 9950–9955. ( 10.1073/pnas.1305814110) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Laakso A, Vilkman H, Kajander J, Bergman J, Haaparanta M, Solin O, Hietala J. 2000. Prediction of detached personality in healthy subjects by low dopamine transporter binding. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 290–292. ( 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.290) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Suhara T, Yasuno F, Sudo Y, Yamamoto M, Inoue M, Okubo Y, Suzuki K. 2001. Dopamine D2 receptors in the insular cortex and the personality trait of novelty seeking. Neuroimage 13, 891–895. ( 10.1006/nimg.2001.0761) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Zald DH, et al. 2008. Midbrain dopamine receptor availability is inversely associated with novelty-seeking traits in humans. J. Neurosci. 28, 14 372–14 378. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2423-08.2008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Gjedde A, Kumakura Y, Cumming P, Linnet J, Möller A, Möller A. 2010. Inverted-U-shaped correlation between dopamine receptor availability in striatum and sensation seeking. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 3870–3875. ( 10.1073/pnas.0912319107) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Bernow N, et al. 2011. Dopamine D2/D3 receptor availability and venturesomeness. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 193, 80–84. ( 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.01.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Oswald LM, et al. 2007. Impulsivity and chronic stress are associated with amphetamine-induced striatal dopamine release. Neuroimage 36, 153–166. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.055) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Buckholtz JW, et al. 2010. Dopaminergic network differences in human impulsivity. Science 329, 532 ( 10.1126/science.1185778) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kestler LP, Malhotra AK, Finch C, Adler C, Breier A. 2000. The relation between dopamine D2 receptor density and personality: preliminary evidence from the NEO personality inventory-revised. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 13, 48–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kim JH, Son YD, Kim HK, Lee SY, Cho SE, Kim YB, Cho Z-H. 2011. Association of harm avoidance with dopamine D2/3 receptor availability in striatal subdivisions: a high resolution PET study. Biol. Psychol. 87, 164–167. ( 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Laakso A, et al. 2003. Personality traits and striatal dopamine synthesis capacity in healthy subjects. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 904–910. ( 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.904) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Stokes PRA, Benecke A, Puraite J, Bloomfield MAP, Shotbolt P, Reeves SJ, Lingford-Hughes AR, Howes O, Egerton A. 2014. Does human presynaptic striatal dopamine function predict social conformity? J. Psychopharmacol. 28, 237–243. ( 10.1177/0269881113512037) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Plavén-Sigray P, Gustavsson P, Farde L, Borg J, Stenkrona P, Nyberg L, Bäckman L, Cervenka S. 2014. Dopamine D1 receptor availability is related to social behavior: a positron emission tomography study. Neuroimage 102, 590–595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Caravaggio F, Fervaha G, Chung JK, Gerretsen P, Nakajima S, Plitman E, Iwata Y, Wilson A, Graff-Guerrero A. 2016. Exploring personality traits related to dopamine D2/3 receptor availability in striatal subregions of humans. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26, 644–652. ( 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.02.010) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Takano A, Arakawa R, Hayashi M, Takahashi H, Ito H, Suhara T. 2007. Relationship between neuroticism personality trait and serotonin transporter binding. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 588–592. ( 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Reimold M, et al. 2008. Anxiety is associated with reduced central serotonin transporter availability in unmedicated patients with unipolar major depression: a [11C] DASB PET study. Mol. Psychiatry 13, 606 ( 10.1038/sj.mp.4002149) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Borg J, Andrée B, Soderstrom H, Farde L. 2003. The serotonin system and spiritual experiences. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1965–1969. ( 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1965) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Moresco FM, et al. 2002. In vivo serotonin 5HT2A receptor binding and personality traits in healthy subjects: a positron emission tomography study. Neuroimage 17, 1470–1478. ( 10.1006/nimg.2002.1239) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Frokjaer VG, et al. 2008. Frontolimbic serotonin 2A receptor binding in healthy subjects is associated with personality risk factors for affective disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 569–576. ( 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Soloff PH, Price JC, Mason NS, Becker C, Meltzer CC. 2010. Gender, personality, and serotonin-2A receptor binding in healthy subjects. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 181, 77–84. ( 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.08.007) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Soloff PH, Chiappetta L, Mason NS, Becker C, Price JC. 2014. Effects of serotonin-2A receptor binding and gender on personality traits and suicidal behavior in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 222, 140–148. ( 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.03.008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Meyer JH, Houle S, Sagrati S, Carella A, Hussey DF, Ginovart N, Goulding V, Kennedy J, Wilson AA. 2004. Brain serotonin transporter binding potential measured with carbon11–labeled DASB positron emission tomography: effects of major depressive episodes and severity of dysfunctional attitudes. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61, 1271–1279. ( 10.1001/archpsyc.61.12.1271) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Parsey RV, Oquendo MA, Simpson NR, Ogden RT, Van Heertum R, Arango V, Mann JJ. 2002. Effects of sex, age, and aggressive traits in man on brain serotonin 5-HT1A receptor binding potential measured by PET using [C-11]WAY-100635. Brain Res. 954, 173–182. ( 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)03243-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.da Cunha-Bang S, Stenbæk DS, Holst K, Licht CL, Jensen PS, Frokjaer VG, Mortensen EL, Knudsen GM. 2013. Trait aggression and trait impulsivity are not related to frontal cortex 5-HT2A receptor binding in healthy individuals. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 212, 125–131. ( 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.09.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.da Cunha-Bang S, Mc Mahon B, MacDonald Fisher P, Jensen PS, Svarer C, Moos Knudsen G. 2016. High trait aggression in men is associated with low 5-HT levels, as indexed by 5-HT4 receptor binding. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 11, 548–555. ( 10.1093/scan/nsv140) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Kalbitzer J, et al. 2009. The personality trait openness is related to cerebral 5-HTT levels. Neuroimage 45, 280–285. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Kim J-H, Son Y-D, Kim J-H, Choi E-J, Lee S-Y, Joo Y-H, Kim Y-B, Cho Z-H. 2015. Self-transcendence trait and its relationship with in vivo serotonin transporter availability in brainstem raphe nuclei: an ultra-high resolution PET-MRI study. Brain Res. 1629, 63–71. ( 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.10.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Karlsson H, Hirvonen J, Salminen JK, Hietala J. 2011. No association between serotonin 5-HT1A receptors and spirituality among patients with major depressive disorders or healthy volunteers. Mol. Psychiatry 16, 282–285. ( 10.1038/mp.2009.126) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Gerretsen P, Graff-Guerrero A, Menon M, Pollock BG, Kapur S, Vasdev N, Houle S, Mamo D. 2010. Is desire for social relationships mediated by the serotonergic system in the prefrontal cortex? An [18F]setoperone PET study. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 375–383. ( 10.1080/17470911003589309) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Takahashi H, et al. 2012. Honesty mediates the relationship between serotonin and reaction to unfairness. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 4281–4284. ( 10.1073/pnas.1118687109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Gorgolewski KJ, Poldrack RA. 2016. A practical guide for improving transparency and reproducibility in neuroimaging research. PLoS Biol. 14, e1002506 ( 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002506) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Munafò MR, et al. 2017. A manifesto for reproducible science. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 21 ( 10.1038/s41562-016-0021) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Gustavsson JP, Asberg M, Schalling D. 1997. The healthy control subject in psychiatric research: impulsiveness and volunteer bias. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 96, 325–328. ( 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1997.tb09924.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Schlagenhauf F, et al. 2013. Ventral striatal prediction error signaling is associated with dopamine synthesis capacity and fluid intelligence. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1490–1499. ( 10.1002/hbm.22000) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Roiser JP, Howes OD, Chaddock CA, Joyce EM, McGuire P. 2013. Neural and behavioral correlates of aberrant salience in individuals at risk for psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 39, 1328–1336. ( 10.1093/schbul/sbs147) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Fletcher PC, Frith CD. 2009. Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 48–58. ( 10.1038/nrn2536) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Berg J, Dickhaut J, McCabe K. 1995. Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games Econ. Behav. 10, 122–142. ( 10.1006/game.1995.1027) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
This article has no additional data.