Significance
We propose genetic instrumental variable (GIV) regression—a method that controls for pleiotropic effects of genes on two variables. GIV regression is broadly applicable to study outcomes for which polygenic scores from large-scale genome-wide association studies are available. We explore the performance of GIV regression in the presence of pleiotropy across a range of scenarios and find that it yields more accurate estimates than alternative approaches such as ordinary least-squares regression or Mendelian randomization. When GIV regression is combined with proper controls for purely environmental sources of bias (e.g., using control variables and sibling fixed effects), it improves our understanding of the causal relationships between genetically correlated variables.
Keywords: causal effects, genetic instrumental variables, polygenic scores, genome-wide association studies, pleiotropy
Abstract
Identifying causal effects in nonexperimental data is an enduring challenge. One proposed solution that recently gained popularity is the idea to use genes as instrumental variables [i.e., Mendelian randomization (MR)]. However, this approach is problematic because many variables of interest are genetically correlated, which implies the possibility that many genes could affect both the exposure and the outcome directly or via unobserved confounding factors. Thus, pleiotropic effects of genes are themselves a source of bias in nonexperimental data that would also undermine the ability of MR to correct for endogeneity bias from nongenetic sources. Here, we propose an alternative approach, genetic instrumental variable (GIV) regression, that provides estimates for the effect of an exposure on an outcome in the presence of pleiotropy. As a valuable byproduct, GIV regression also provides accurate estimates of the chip heritability of the outcome variable. GIV regression uses polygenic scores (PGSs) for the outcome of interest which can be constructed from genome-wide association study (GWAS) results. By splitting the GWAS sample for the outcome into nonoverlapping subsamples, we obtain multiple indicators of the outcome PGSs that can be used as instruments for each other and, in combination with other methods such as sibling fixed effects, can address endogeneity bias from both pleiotropy and the environment. In two empirical applications, we demonstrate that our approach produces reasonable estimates of the chip heritability of educational attainment (EA) and show that standard regression and MR provide upwardly biased estimates of the effect of body height on EA.
A major challenge in the social sciences and in epidemiology is the identification of causal effects in nonexperimental data. In these disciplines, ethical and legal considerations along with practical constraints often preclude the use of experiments to randomize the assignment of observations between treatment and control groups or to carry out such experiments in samples that represent the relevant population (1). Instead, many important questions are studied in field data which make it difficult to discern between causal effects and (spurious) correlations that are induced by unobserved factors (2). Obviously, confusing correlation with causation is not only a conceptual error; it can also lead to ineffective or even harmful recommendations, treatments, and policies, as well as a significant waste of resources (e.g., as in ref. 3).
One important source of bias in field data stems from genetic effects: Twin studies (4) as well as methods based on molecular genetic data (5, 6) allow estimation of the proportion of variance in a trait that is due to linear genetic effects (so-called narrow-sense heritability). Using these and related methods, an overwhelming body of literature demonstrates that almost all important human characteristics, behaviors, and health outcomes are influenced both by genetic predisposition and by environmental factors (7–9). Most of these traits are “genetically complex,” which means that the observed heritability is due to the accumulation of effects from a very large number of genes that each have a small, often statistically insignificant, influence (10).
Furthermore, genes often influence several seemingly unrelated traits, a phenomenon called direct or vertical pleiotropy (11, 12). For example, a mutation of a single gene that causes the disease phenylketonuria is responsible for mental retardation and also for abnormally light hair and skin color (13). Pleiotropy is not restricted to diseases. All genes involved in healthy cell metabolism and cell division can be expected to directly influence a broad range of traits such as body height, cognitive ability, and longevity, even if the effect on each of these traits may be tiny. Similarly, any gene involved in neurodevelopment and brain function is likely to contribute to human behavior and mental health in some way (14).
In addition to direct pleiotropic effects, genes can also have indirect or horizontal pleiotropic effects, where a genetic variant influences one trait, which in turn influences another trait (11). The similarity of the genetic architecture of two traits is estimated by their genetic correlation (i.e., the correlation of the “true” effect sizes of all genetic variants on both traits) (15), which captures both direct and indirect pleiotropic effects (16–18). Genetic correlations exist between many traits and often exceed their phenotypic correlations (19), giving rise to the concern that direct pleiotropy may substantially bias studies that do not control for genetic effects (20).
If an experimental design is not possible, the gold standard in the presence of genetic confounds is to compare outcomes for monozygotic (MZ) twins (21, 22), who are by definition genetically (almost) identical (23). In addition, this approach also controls for effects that arise from shared parental environment. However, the practical challenge is that such studies require very large sample sizes of MZ twin pairs because differences within MZ twin pairs tend to be small or nonexistent. Furthermore, unobserved environmental differences between the twins or reverse causation can still lead to wrong conclusions in this study design.
Another popular strategy to isolate causal effects in nonexperimental data is to use instrumental variables (IVs) (24). Valid IVs are conceptually similar to natural experiments: They provide an exogenous “shock” on the exposure of interest to isolate the effect of that exposure on an outcome. Valid IVs need to satisfy two important conditions. First, they need to be correlated with the exposure conditional on the other control variables in the regression (i.e., IVs need to be “relevant”). Second, they need to be independent of the error term of the regression conditional on the other control variables and produce their correlation with the outcome solely through their effect on the exposure (the so-called exclusion restriction). In practice, finding valid IVs that satisfy both requirements is difficult. In particular, satisfying the exclusion restriction is challenging. [Two other conditions that valid IVs need to satisfy are monotonicity (everyone who is affected by the IV is affected in the same direction) and the stable unit treatment value assumption (SUTVA): The “treatment” of one unit does not affect the outcome variable for other units.]
Epidemiologists have proposed to use genetic information to construct IVs and termed this approach Mendelian randomization (MR) (25–28). The idea is in principle appealing because genotypes are randomized in the production of gametes by the process of meiosis. Thus, conditional on the genotype of the parents, the genotype of the offspring is the result of a random draw. So if it could be known which genes affect the exposure, it may be possible to use them as IVs to identify the causal influence of the exposure on some outcome of interest. However, there are four challenges to this idea. First, we need to know which genes affect the exposure and isolate true genetic effects from environmental confounds that are correlated with ancestry. Second, if the exposure is a genetically complex trait, any gene by itself will capture only a very small part of the variance in the trait, which leads to the well-known problem of weak instruments (29, 30). Third, genotypes are randomly assigned only conditional on the genotype of the parents. Unless it is possible to control for the genotype of the parents, the genotype of the offspring is not random and correlates with everything that the genotypes of the parents correlate with (e.g., parental environment, personality, and habits) (31). Fourth, if direct pleiotropic effects of genes are the source of the confound, these genes could obviously not be used as IVs. One could try to isolate a subset of genes that influence only the exposure, but such attempts are still hindered by our limited knowledge of the function of most genes (27, 32, 33).
Recent advances in complex trait genetics make it possible to address the first two challenges of MR. Array-based genotyping technologies have made the collection of genetic data fast and cheap. As a result, very large datasets are now available to study the genetic architecture of many human traits and a plethora of robust, replicable genetic associations have recently been reported in large-scale genome-wide association studies (GWASs) (34). These results begin to shed light on the genetic architecture that is driving the heritability of traits such as body height (35), body mass index (BMI) (36), schizophrenia (37), Alzheimer’s disease (38), depression (39), and educational attainment (EA) (15).
High-quality GWASs use several strategies to control for genetic structure in the population, and empirical evidence suggests that the vast majority of the reported genetic associations for many traits is not confounded by ancestry (40–43). Polygenic scores (PGSs) have become the favored tool for summarizing the genetic predispositions for genetically complex traits (15, 39, 44, 45). PGSs are linear indexes that aggregate the estimated effects of all currently measured genetic variants [typically single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs)]. The effects of each SNP on an outcome are estimated in large-scale GWASs that exclude the prediction sample. Recent studies demonstrate that this approach yields PGSs that begin to predict genetically complex outcomes such as height, BMI, schizophrenia, and EA (35–37, 39, 46). Although PGSs still capture substantially less of the variation in traits than suggested by their heritability (47) (an issue we return to below), PGSs capture a much larger share of the variance of genetically complex traits than individual genetic markers. The third challenge to MR in the above list could in principle be addressed if the genotypes of the parents and the offspring are observed (e.g., in a large sample of parent–offspring trios) or by using large samples of siblings or dizygotic twins where the genetic differences between siblings are random draws from the parent’s genotypes. However, the fourth challenge (i.e., pleiotropy) remains a serious obstacle despite recent efforts to relax the exogeneity assumptions in MR (48–50).
Here, we address the implications of pleiotropy for modeling causal relationships using nonexperimental data. We demonstrate that pleiotropy is a serious source of bias in ordinary least-squares regression (OLS) and MR. We propose alternative estimation strategies that use PGSs for the outcome of interest to reduce bias arising from pleiotropy. In particular, we propose an approach that we call genetic instrumental variables (GIV) regression that can be implemented using widely available statistical software. GIV regression estimates practically useful upper and lower bounds for the causal effect of an exposure on an outcome even in the presence of substantial direct pleiotropy.
We begin by providing intuition and laying out the assumptions of our approach. We go on to show that GIV regression produces accurate estimates for the effect of the PGSs on the outcome variable when the other covariates in the model are exogenous, when the true PGS is uncorrelated with the error term net of the included covariates, and when the GWAS sample sizes are sufficiently large relative to the number of SNPs. We then turn to the more complex case of when a regressor of interest is potentially correlated with unobserved variables in the error term because of pleiotropy, and we show with evidence from a comprehensive set of simulations that the bias under these assumptions with GIV regression is generally smaller than with OLS, MR, or what we term an enhanced version of MR (EMR).
Next, we demonstrate the practical usefulness of our approach in empirical applications using the publicly available Health and Retirement Study (HRS) (51). First, we demonstrate that a consistent estimate of the so-called chip heritability (47) of EA can be obtained with our method. Then, we estimate the effects of body height on EA. As a “negative control,” we check whether our method finds a causal effect of EA on body height (it should not). (Note that a clean experimental design which randomizes people into groups based on body height or EA is not possible. Thus, any attempt to study the causal relationship between the two variables must rely on observational data and naturally occurring experiments like the genetic endowment of individuals, which we exploit here.)
Formal derivations and technical details are contained in SI Appendix, sections 1–5.
Theory
Intuition.
To build intuition for our approach, we introduce the concept of the true PGS for which would be constructed using the true effects of each SNP on . In theory, the true SNP effects could be estimated in a GWAS on in an infinitely large sample that is drawn from the same population as the prediction sample. The true PGS would capture the narrow-sense heritability of . Of course, the true PGS is unknown. All one can practically obtain is a PGS from a finite GWAS sample that will capture a part, but not all, of the genetic influence on because the effect of each SNP is estimated with noise. The attenuated predictive accuracy of practically available PGSs (45, 47, 52) is conceptually similar to the well-known problem of measurement error in regression analysis. It has long been understood that multiple indicators can, under certain conditions, provide a strategy to correct regression estimates for attenuation from measurement error (53–55). We show below that by splitting the GWAS sample into independent subsamples, one can obtain several PGSs (i.e., multiple indicators) in the prediction sample. Each will have even lower predictive accuracy than the original score due to the smaller GWAS subsamples used in their construction, but these multiple indicators can be used as instrumental variables for each other, and the instruments will satisfy the assumptions of IV regression to the extent that the measurement errors (the difference between the true and calculated PGSs) are uncorrelated. Standard two-stage least-squares (2SLS) regression (24) (readily available in statistics software packages) using at least one valid IV for the PGS of can then be used to back out an unbiased estimate of the heritability of .
Next, presume the matter of interest is not heritability, but the causal effect of some treatment on , where is also heritable and some genes have direct pleiotropic effects on both. If these genes are not known and not controlled for, regressing on would result in omitted variable bias. (Unfortunately, that is the reality in most social scientific and epidemiological studies that use nonexperimental data.) Suppose the effects of all genes that influence through channels other than could be known. Theoretically, one could estimate these effects in a GWAS on that controls for in an infinitely large sample. That information could be used to construct a “true conditional” PGS in a prediction sample. Adding the true conditional PGS to a regression of on in the prediction sample would effectively eliminate bias arising from direct pleiotropy. However, the true conditional PGS is also unknown and all we can practically obtain is a noisy proxy of it from a finite GWAS sample. While it is not guaranteed, the general conclusion of the literature is that the use of proxy variables is an improvement over omitting the variable being proxied (56, 57). Furthermore, having a valid IV for the conditional score would potentially correct for its noise and get us closer to estimating the true causal effect of on . As before, a valid IV can be practically obtained by splitting the GWAS sample into independent parts and standard IV estimation techniques such as two-stage least squares can be used. We refer to this approach as conditional genetic IV regression (GIV-C).
If conditional GWAS results are not available, one can still add the unconditional PGS for as a control variable and use IV regression with multiple indicators for this score to correct for measurement error. We refer to this as unconditional GIV regression (GIV-U). GIV-U still corrects for bias arising from direct pleiotropy, but this strategy will overcontrol and result in estimates for that are biased toward zero because the unconditional PGS also includes indirect pleiotropic effects of genes that affect only because they affect . However, extensive simulations show that the combination of GIV-C and GIV-U turns out to produce reasonable upper and lower bounds for the effect of on across a broad range of scenarios if the only sources of bias are pleiotropic genes.
The GIV strategy starts to break down when bias arises from unobserved nongenetic factors as well as from pleiotropic effects. We show below that both GIV and MR produce biased estimates in this case. However, we demonstrate that the combination of GIV-C and GIV-U still outperforms OLS and MR. Furthermore, the GIV approach has additional utility because it can be combined with other strategies to reduce the effects of environmental endogeneity (e.g., additional control variables or family fixed effects). We demonstrate that these combined strategies can potentially provide accurate information about the effects of an exposure in situations with both genetic and nongenetic sources of endogeneity. In contrast, the problems for MR that are produced by pleiotropy bias are not fixable in a similar manner.
Assumptions.
GIV regression builds on the standard identifying assumptions of IV regression (24). In the context of our approach, this implies six specific conditions:
-
i)
Polygenicity: The outcome is a genetically complex trait that is influenced by many genetic variants, each with a very small effect.
-
ii)
Complete genetic information: The available genetic data include all variants that influence the variable(s) of interest.
-
iii)
Genetic effects are linear: All genetic variants influence the variable(s) of interest via additive linear effects. Thus, there are no genetic interactions (i.e., epistasis) or dominant alleles.
-
iv)
Unbiased GWAS results: The available GWAS results are not systematically biased by omitted environmental variables. For example, failure to control for population structure can lead to spurious genetic associations (31).
-
v)
Nonoverlapping samples: It is possible to divide GWAS samples into nonoverlapping subsamples drawn from the same population.
-
vi)
The genetic effects on are the same in the GWAS and the prediction samples; i.e., the genetic correlation between samples is one.
Estimating Narrow-Sense SNP Heritability from Polygenic Scores.
Under these assumptions, consistent estimates of the chip heritability of a trait (i.e., the proportion of variance in a trait that is due to linear effects of currently measurable SNPs) can be obtained from polygenic scores (for full details, see SI Appendix, section 2). If is the outcome variable, is a vector of exogenous control variables, and is a summary measure of genetic tendency for in the presence of controls for , then one can write
[1] |
where is an matrix of genetic markers, and is the vector of SNP effect sizes, where the number of SNPs is typically in the millions. If the true effects of each SNP on the outcome were known, the entire genetic tendency for would be captured by the true unconditional score , and the marginal of in Eq. 1 would be the chip heritability of the trait. We refer to the estimate of the PGS from actually available GWAS data in the presence of controls for as , where
[2] |
and is the estimation error in and is substituted for in Eq. 1. The variance of a trait that is captured by its available PGS increases with the available GWAS sample size to estimate and converges to the SNP-based narrow-sense heritability of the trait at the limit if all relevant genetic markers were included in the GWAS and if the GWAS sample size were sufficiently large (45, 47, 52).
Eq. 1 contains what is called in econometrics a “generated regressor” in that is a function of a set of variables and coefficients () from another model. As previous work (58, 59) has established, OLS will provide consistent estimates of the parameters of Eq. 1 (although corrections for standard errors are needed) if is substituted for under a set of reasonable assumptions that include convergence in probability of to as the sample size grows larger. However, the practical utility of this mathematical result is questionable in the current context, when the number of variables in is in the millions while the number of cases available to estimate is far smaller than that. The imposed ratio of coefficients to cases requires nonconventional estimation methods that use a combination of statistical assumptions to obtain estimates of . Empirical studies using PGSs for a variety of traits have consistently demonstrated substantial attenuation in the estimate of (45, 47, 52), and, while the bias diminishes with GWAS sample size, we are a long time away from having large enough sample sizes to bring this attenuation down to ignorable levels. This situation, therefore, calls for alternative strategies to address important questions with the datasets currently available.
The most straightforward solution to the problem of attenuation bias is to obtain multiple indicators of the PGS by splitting the GWAS discovery sample for into two mutually exclusive subsamples with at least partially overlapping sets of SNPs. This produces noisier estimates of with lower predictive accuracy, but the multiple indicators can be used as IVs for each other. The 2SLS regression using as an instrument for will then recover a consistent estimate of in Eq. 1 under standard IV assumptions (55, 60).
As discussed more technically in SI Appendix, section 1, an additional important assumption for to be a valid instrument for is that be a complex trait, meaning that it is influenced by a large number of genetic markers, each of which has a very small effect. If is primarily influenced by a relatively small number of markers, then the method proposed here would not work well. However, there would also be no need for the proposed method, because the markers with large effects could be easily identified and their effects estimated with reasonable precision using discovery sample sizes that are already obtainable.
Another required assumption is that the genetic markers are independent of each other. In general, genetic markers are correlated if they are located close to each other on the same chromosome. However, it is currently possible to isolate several hundred thousand markers from the total set of millions of SNPs that have sufficient spatial separation in the DNA to be essentially mutually independent, which means that this assumption can be satisfied to a sufficient level of accuracy.
Assuming that the variables in Eq. 1 are standardized to have mean zero and a SD of one, and further assuming that the variables contained in control for population stratification or are not correlated with genotype , a consistent estimate of the chip heritability of can now be obtained from , where is the correlation coefficient. The heritability estimate is not equal to simply because we regressed on instead of . Thus, we standardize with respect to the variance of instead of , which leads to a bias equal to . Multiplying with the correlation between and recovers a consistent estimate for (SI Appendix, section 2.1). (For an alternative approach to correcting attenuation bias based on the use of multiple indicators in a structural equation modeling framework, see ref. 61.)
Reducing Bias Arising from Genetic Correlation Between Exposure and Outcome.
Polygenic scores also play a potentially important role in situations where the question of interest is not the chip heritability of per se, but rather the effect of some nonrandomized exposure on (e.g., a behavioral or environmental variable or a nonrandomized treatment due to policy or medical interventions). We can rewrite Eq. 1 by adding a treatment variable of interest , such that
[3] |
where
[4] |
We assume that the disturbance term is uncorrelated with genetic variables. (We drop this assumption later. Also, we drop the subscript on the coefficients for the exogenous control variables below when it would not lead to confusion.) We now use the true conditional score rather than in the equation. Given that is in the model, the effect of individual SNPs on will generally involve a direct net effect of and an indirect effect stemming from the combination of their effect on and the effect of on Having in the equation would effectively control for pleiotropic effects on and .
In standard MR, a measure of genetic tendency () for a behavior of interest ( in Eq. 3) is used as an IV in an effort to purge of bias that arises from correlation between and unobservable variables in the disturbance term under the assumption that is exogenous (60, 62). One such example would be the use of a PGS for height as an instrument for height in a regression of EA on height. The problem with this approach is that the PGS for height will fail to satisfy the exclusion restriction if (some of) the genes affecting height also have a direct effect on EA (e.g., via healthy cell growth and metabolism) or if they are correlated with unmeasured environmental factors that affect EA. (Classic MR typically does not use PGSs as instruments. Instead, the idea is to use single genetic variants that are known to affect the exposure via well-understood biological mechanisms that make it unlikely to violate the exclusion restriction. In practice, limited knowledge about the biological function of most genes makes it difficult to argue that direct pleiotropic effects of the gene on the exposure and the outcome of interest exist.)
If the true conditional (net of ) genetic propensity for could be directly controlled in the regression, pleiotropy would not bias coefficient estimates. For example, fixed-effects regression where the same individual is observed multiple times would effectively control for pleiotropy (which does not vary over time), but this strategy is often not available (e.g., in a study of the effect of height on EA).* Direct control for the conditional genetic propensity for is, of course, not possible, because (more specifically, the coefficients, in Eq. 3) is not known. What is obtainable instead is a proxy for , namely , which contains measurement error due to finite GWAS sample size and potential bias in the estimate of in the GWAS.
We refer to the combined use of as a control and as an IV for as EMR. However, controlling for as a proxy for is not a perfect solution to pleiotropy because it leaves a component of in the error term which is correlated with due to pleiotropy. As a result, the EMR estimate for will be biased. The practical question, then, is whether alternative strategies that split the GWAS sample for to obtain multiple indicators of that can be used as IVs for each other (e.g., and ) are sufficient to rescue as a practically useful IV for .† This is a practical question beyond the reach of formal mathematics and best answered by simulation analyses. Unfortunately, and as we show in SI Appendix, section 2, the pleiotropy-induced violation of the exclusion restriction when using genetic IVs for is sufficient to produce serious bias in the estimated effect of even if one attempts to control for pleiotropy using such strategies. The magnitude of bias clearly depends on the quality of the proxies for . However, we find that pleiotropy leads to considerable bias in MR in virtually all scenarios we investigated (SI Appendix, sections 2, 3, and 4 and Tables S2–S15).
The problems posed by pleiotropy cannot be completely eliminated without knowledge of (SI Appendix, section 2). However, this situation does not mean that the estimation problems introduced by pleiotropy are intractable. When endogeneity bias is driven by genetic correlation, the PGS for can still be used to obtain more accurate estimates of the effect of than can be obtained with MR or, for that matter, with OLS that lacks controls for the direct effect of genetic markers on . To gain insight into the best strategy, we consider the reasons for the pleiotropy bias. Regardless of whether the estimation strategy is OLS or the second stage of IV regression involving OLS on predicted variables from the first stage, the coefficient bias comes from the extent to which the expected estimate of the OLS coefficients differs from the true coefficients;‡ namely,
[5] |
In other words, the coefficient bias from OLS is the expected regression coefficient of the error on the included variables in the regression. If is the sum of an omitted variable, , which is correlated with the regressors, and additional variables that are uncorrelated with the regressors, then the bias for each coefficient in Eq. 5 becomes the product of the regression coefficient for in the regression of on all of the omitted variables multiplied by the effect of on the outcome. For simplicity, we assume that the only variables in the regression are and a potential proxy for , which we call . For any given proxy, , the bias in the estimate of (the coefficient for in Eq. 3) comes from the expected coefficient of from a regression of on and . We consider three alternative approaches for the proxy , which we call simple OLS, GIV-C, and GIV-U. First, we use as a proxy for in a simple OLS regression. Second, we observe that is correlated with ; its inclusion in the error (by virtue of its being controlled) may affect the bias in . So we construct an estimate for , namely , by using (the unconditional PGS from the second GWAS sample) as its IV. We call this approach, where is used as the regressor in the second stage, GIV-C. We also use a third estimator that uses the same IV as in GIV-C (i.e., ), but that substitutes the unconditional PGS for (i.e., substitutes for the conditional PGS ) in the structural model in Eq. 3. We then use to predict , obtaining as the regressor in the second stage. We call this third approach GIV-U.
We generally expect the use of GIV-C to perform better than the use of the proxy in simple OLS. If the true effect of on is positive and positive pleiotropy is present, the estimated effect of on will have positive bias. This follows from the positive correlation between and and from the positive effect of on . The presence of the proxy in the first approach (simple OLS) adds a partially offsetting negative bias, because the correlation between and is positive and the effect is also positive, but is being subtracted, which causes the offsetting bias to be negative. The net bias is expected to be positive, but we would expect it to be smaller with the inclusion of the proxy than with no proxy at all, both because the correlation between and would be lower than between and and because we expect to be attenuated relative to . When GIV-C is used instead, the term in the error becomes . The presence in the first stage of GIV-C of , which is correlated with , prevents the IV strategy from obtaining a consistent estimate of . Nonetheless, we would generally expect , and therefore we expect the positive bias for the estimate of to be smaller when using GIV-C than when estimating using OLS and the proxy . We confirm this in the simulations in SI Appendix, Tables S2–S7.
With GIV-U, the problem term in the error is . As before, the presence of the first term produces a positive bias in the estimate of , while the second term produces an offsetting negative bias. The offset will be stronger when the unconditional PGS for is the regressor in the structural model, because the coefficients of the genetic markers in are , where is the effect of the genetic marker on . The presence of in the second endogenous term in the error (i.e., the second term in ) produces a stronger downward bias. This downward bias is made still stronger by the use of instead of as the coefficient, because we expect the first-stage regression to reduce the downward bias of . In other words, we expect these three proxies to behave differently in the simulations, and, as we will see, this expectation is met in practice. We establish via a comprehensive set of simulations that GIV-C and GIV-U provide upper and lower bounds for the effect of across a range of plausible scenarios for pleiotropy and for heritability (SI Appendix, Tables S2–S7). We further establish through simulation analyses that GIV-C and GIV-U perform similarly in the case when endogeneity arises from pleiotropy and when it arises from pleiotropy in combination with genetic confounds for reasons other than pleiotropy [epistasis, effects from rare alleles, or genetic nurturing effects, where the environment of ego is shaped by genetically related individuals to ego (63)] (SI Appendix, section 3 and Tables S8–S10).
Simulations
We explored the performance of GIV regression in finite sample sizes using three sets of simulation scenarios (SI Appendix, sections 2–4). The simulations generated genetic and phenotypic data at the individual level from a set of known models in a training sample and a holdout sample using parameters that are realistic for genetically complex traits. We then estimated genetic effects on and using GWAS in the training sample and constructed polygenic scores with the estimated parameters for each SNP in the holdout sample. Thus, the polygenic scores in our simulations have the realistic property that their predictive accuracy increases with the size of the training (i.e., GWAS) sample and the average effect size of each SNP (45, 52). Finally, we analyzed the extent to which various estimation strategies recover the effect of the PGS for on and the effect of on in the holdout sample. We produced these estimates using OLS, MR, EMR, proxy OLS, GIV-C, and GIV-U regression, and we compared these results with the true answer across a range of parameter values. We ran 20 simulations with different random seeds for each set of parameters to obtain a distribution of estimated effects. (The computer code for these simulations is available at https://github.com/cburik/GIVsim.)
The simulations specify that the true PGS scores for and covary as a result of genetic correlation. We made the conservative assumption that the entire genetic correlation between and is due to direct pleiotropy; i.e., all genes that are associated with both phenotypes have direct effects on both. In practice, this is unlikely to be the case, but it is equally unlikely that one can put a credible upper bound on (or completely rule out) direct pleiotropy.
In the first set of simulations, we assumed that the entire endogeneity problem arises from genetic confounds (SI Appendix, section 2).
In the second set of simulations, we allowed endogeneity to arise from sources that are both correlated with genes and that cause the disturbance term in the structural equation for to be correlated with the disturbance term in the structural model for even if the true conditional PGS for were included in the structural equation (SI Appendix, section 3). This situation would occur if rare alleles were missing from the true PGS for and the true conditional PGS for based on known SNPs and if the effect of these alleles was correlated with the true PGS scores for and . It would also occur under conditions of epistasis where nonlinear effects of genes were in the error and were correlated with the linear effects of genes in the PGS for and the conditional PGS for Third, this situation would occur if the genetic factors that affect are correlated with the environmental factors in the disturbance term for that are caused or selected by parental genes, which are correlated with the genes of sample members and therefore also with variables (like ) that are affected by the genes of sample members, i.e., by genetic nurturing (63).
In the third set of simulations, we specified the presence of a correlation between the error terms in the models for and that was not itself correlated with genetic variables (SI Appendix, section 4). This would occur in a situation where some environmental or behavioral factor that is unrelated to genetics produces both an effect on and an effect on .
A summary of these results is in Table 1 for the case where the effect of is set to 1.0 (see SI Appendix, Table S16 for details on the standardized effect size). Scenarios A–D in Table 1 refer to situations where pleiotropy is the only source of bias, scenario E contains pleiotropy plus other sources of genetic confounds, while scenarios F and G also include endogeneity from nongenetic (i.e., environmental) sources. The results provide considerable reason to be skeptical of estimates from MR. When pleiotropy is present, the MR strategy is undermined by the violation of the exclusion restriction for genetic IVs. Our results find that MR performs poorly even when nongenetic endogeneity is present along with pleiotropy. In contrast, GIV regression provides reasonable upper and lower bounds of the true effect of on if the source of endogeneity is only from pleiotropy or other genetic confounds (i.e., unobserved genetic variants, epistasis, or genetic nurturing) and the heritability of and is not extreme. GIV-C generally overestimates the effect of but the overestimation is modest at low to moderate levels of pleiotropy and heritability and is more accurate than OLS without proxies, MR, or EMR. GIV-U generally underestimates the effect of on but provides an estimate that is reasonably close to the true answer under conditions of low to moderate levels of pleiotropy and heritability. Even at higher levels of pleiotropy and heritability, the combination of GIV-C and GIV-U provides useful information about whether actually has a causal effect on and what the upper bound of this effect is likely to be.
Table 1.
Model | Parameter | OLS | MR | GIV-C | GIV-U |
A | Pleiotropy only | 1.1004 | 1.5040 | 1.0131 | 0.9419 |
(0.0001) | (0.0012) | (0.0001) | (0.0001) | ||
B | Pleiotropy only | 1.2011 | 1.5024 | 1.0604 | 0.8575 |
(0.0001) | (0.0004) | (0.0001) | (0.0001) | ||
C | Pleiotropy only | 1.3016 | 1.5020 | 1.1573 | 0.7263 |
(0.0001) | (0.0002) | (0.0001) | (0.0002) | ||
D | Pleiotropy only | 1.5004 | 3.4922 | 1.0776 | 0.8689 |
, | (0.0005) | (0.0093) | (0.0002) | (0.0002) | |
E | Genetic Confounds | 1.3106 | 1.4609 | 1.1922 | 0.6422 |
, | (0.0001) | (0.0002) | (0.0001) | (0.0002) | |
F | Nongenetic confounds | 1.4520 | 1.5032 | 1.4259 | 1.1193 |
, | (0.0001) | (0.0002) | (0.0001) | (0.0001) | |
G | Nongenetic confounds with control | 1.3643 | 1.5064 | 1.3346 | 0.9587 |
, , | (0.0001) | (0.0002) | (0.0001) | (0.0001) |
Shown are mean estimated coefficient for T and SE within parentheses of 20 simulations using different estimation methods for several models. For all models the genetic correlation () was 0.5 and the coefficient for T () was 1. and are the heritability parameters of y and T. is the correlation between and the genetic confound for . is the correlation between and the genetic confound for . is the correlation between the nongenetic confound and . is the share of the confound that is controlled for in terms of variance of the confound. These results are a selection from SI Appendix, Tables S2–S4, S7, S9, S11, and S14; see SI Appendix, sections 2–4 for all results. A, Table S2; B, Table S3; C, Table S4; D, Table S7; E, Table S9; F, Table S11; and G, Table S14. See row 2 of each table. See SI Appendix, Table S6 for more details on the parameters, variance, and standardized effect size.
In the case where the true value of is zero (i.e., where the true model is Eq. 1), we expect that GIV-U will produce an estimate that is close to zero as long as the endogeneity comes either from pleiotropy or from other genetic confounds (see SI Appendix, sections 2–4, for details). The simulations in SI Appendix section 2 show that when the endogeneity is only from genetic sources, GIV-U estimates the effect of to be close to zero regardless of the level of pleiotropy or inheritance that is specified in the simulations.
When the source of endogeneity is nongenetic in origin, we find that neither MR nor proxy controls for pleiotropy provide a satisfactory method for determining the effect of on . In this scenario, the pleiotropy creates endogeneity bias for genetic IVs that defeats the ability of MR to solve the problem of nongenetic endogeneity via an IV strategy. Nongenetic endogeneity can cause even GIV-U to overpredict the effect of on although in our simulations it is clearly the most accurate of all of the estimators that we have surveyed when the effect of is zero. Indeed, GIV-U always provides the most conservative estimate across the entire range of scenarios that we have surveyed, both for the case where an effect of on exists and when the effect of is zero.
Inference with the GIV can be further strengthened in cases where nongenetic endogeneity can be controlled either through observable variables or through strategies such as family fixed effects that reduce or eliminate the impact of nongenetic forms of environmental endogeneity. Indeed, environmental endogeneity is a concern in most applied-research questions that use nonexperimental data. Reassuringly, our simulations show that GIV regression is a good estimation strategy in the presence of both direct pleiotropy and environmental endogeneity if control variables are available that manage to absorb a substantial share of the nongenetic confounds (SI Appendix, Tables S13–S15). Therefore, we recommend using GIV regression always in combination with control variables the capture possible environmental confounds, ideally in datasets that allow controlling for family fixed effects (e.g., using siblings or dizygotic twins). In SI Appendix, section 6, we provide additional practical guidelines for GIV regression.
The simulations described in this paper certainly do not cover all conceivable data-generating processes, but they are nonetheless of considerable utility.
Empirical Applications
We illustrate the practical use of GIV regression in two empirical applications using data from the Health and Retirement Survey (HRS) for 2,751 unrelated individuals of northwest European descent who were born between 1935 and 1945 (SI Appendix, section 5).
The Narrow-Sense SNP Heritability of EA.
First, we demonstrate that GIV regression can recover the unbiased genomic–relatedness-matrix restricted maximum-likelihood (GREML) estimate of the chip heritability of EA. Specifically, we follow the common practice in GREML estimates of heritability and analyze the residual of EA from a regression of EA on birth year, birth year squared, gender, and the first 20 principal components from the genetic data (64). Next, we standardize the residual and regress it on a standardized PGS for EA using OLS or GIV. The results are displayed in Table 2. The OLS estimate of the PGS accounts for 6.8% of the variance in EA ( = = 6.8%), which is substantially lower than the 17.3% (95% CI ± 4%) estimate of chip heritability reported by ref. 64 in the same data using GREML. [GREML yields unbiased estimates of SNP-based heritability that are not affected by attenuation (65).] Instead, the GIV regression results in columns 2 and 3 of Table 2 imply a chip heritability of 13.4% (CI ± 3.9%) and 13.8% (±4.0%), respectively. Thus, the 95% CIs of the GREML estimate and the two GIV estimates overlap, demonstrating that GIV regression can recover the chip heritability of EA from polygenic scores.
Table 2.
Variable | OLS | IV1 | IV2 |
PGS EA UKB | 0.259*** | 0.523*** | |
(0.0183) | (0.0385) | ||
PGS EA SSGAC | 0.530*** | ||
(0.0389) | |||
NA | 0.134 | 0.138 | |
NA | (0.0197) | (0.0202) | |
2,751 | 2,751 | 2,751 |
*, **, ***. We regress the residual of EA on the different PGSs and calculate the implied heritability estimates. SEs are in parentheses. All variables have been standardized. EA is measured in years of schooling needed to obtain the highest achieved educational degree according to International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) classifications. We use the residual of EA after a regression on birth year, birth year squared, gender, and the first 20 principal components in the genetic data. PGS EA SSGAC: PGS for EA using meta-analysis from ref. 15, excluding data from 23andMe, UK Biobank (UKB), and HRS; PGS EA UKB, PGS for EA using UKB data. IV1 uses PGS EA SSGAC as instrument and IV2 uses PGS EA UKB as instrument. NA, not applicable.
The Relationship Between Body Height and Educational Attainment.
Previous studies using both OLS and sibling or twin fixed-effects methods have found that taller people generally have higher levels of EA (66–68). They are also more likely to perform well in various other life domains, including earnings, higher marriage rates for men (although with higher probabilities of divorce), and higher fertility (69–74). The question is what drives these results. Can they be attributed to genetic effects that jointly influence these outcomes? Are there social mechanisms that systematically favor taller or penalize shorter individuals? Or are there nongenetic factors (e.g., the uterine and postbirth environments especially related to nutrition or disease) that affect both height and these life-course outcomes? The literature on the relationship between height and EA has found evidence that the association arises largely through the relationship between height and cognitive ability, which may suggest that the height–EA association is driven largely by genetic association between height and cognitive ability. We use GIV regression with individual-level data from the HRS to clarify the influence of height on EA, and we compare these results with those obtained from OLS and from MR. In addition, we conduct a negative control experiment that estimates the causal effect of EA on body height (which should be zero). A complete description of the materials and methods is available in SI Appendix, section 5.
GWAS summary statistics for height were obtained from the Genetic Investigation of Anthropometric Traits (GIANT) consortium (35) and by running a GWAS on height (conditional on EA and unconditional on EA) in the UKB (75). The UKB was not part of the GIANT sample. GWAS summary statistics for EA were obtained from the Social Science Genetic Association Consortium (SSGAC) for the unconditional PGSs. The most recent study of the SSGAC on EA used a meta-analysis of 64 cohorts for genetic discovery (15). We obtained meta-analysis results from this study with the HRS, UKB, and 23andMe cohorts excluded and we refer to the PGS constructed from these results as EA SSGAC. Furthermore, we obtained GWAS estimates for EA in the full UKB release () from ref. 76. We refer to this PGS as PGS EA unc. UKB. We also created a PGS for EA conditional on height by running a GWAS on EA in the same UKB sample (PGS EA cond. UKB). There is sample overlap between Height_GIANT and EA_SSGAC. Therefore, whenever one of the two was used as regressor, we excluded the other as instrument and used a PGS from UK Biobank data instead to ensure independence of measurement errors in the PGS.
In Table 3 we report the estimated standardized effect of height on EA. The OLS results show that height appears to have a strong positive effect on EA, with 2.5 additional centimeters in height generating one additional month of schooling. MR appears to confirm the causal interpretation of the OLS result; indeed, the point estimate from MR is even slightly larger than from OLS. As discussed above, MR suffers from probable violations of the exclusion restriction due to pleiotropy. These violations could stem from the possibility that some genes have direct effects on both height and EA. (Results from refs. 15 and 18 suggest a genetic correlation between height and EA of about 0.15.) They could also stem from the possibility that the PGS for height by itself is correlated with the genetic tendency for parents to have higher EA and income, which enables higher parental investments into their children who may therefore be more likely to reach their full cognitive potential and have higher EA. Controlling for the PGS is an imperfect strategy for eliminating this source of endogeneity because the bias in the estimated effect of the PGS score also biases the estimated effect of height.
Table 3.
Variable | OLS | MR | GIV-C | GIV-U |
Height | 0.136*** | 0.160*** | 0.168*** | 0.110*** |
(0.0262) | (0.0481) | (0.0264) | (0.0262) | |
PGS EA cond. UKB | 0.396*** | |||
(0.0367) | ||||
PGS EA uncond. UKB | 0.384*** | |||
(0.0354) | ||||
2,751 | 2,751 | 2,751 | 2,751 |
*, **, ***. Standardized effect sizes and SEs are in parentheses. Birth year, birth year squared, gender, EA mother, EA father, and the first 20 principal components are included as control variables. For MR, a PGS for height from UK Biobank (UKB) data was used as instrument for height. For GIV-C and GIV-U PGS EA SSGAC was used as an instrument.
If all of the bias in EA came from positive pleiotropy, then we would expect GIV-C and GIV-U to provide upper and lower bounds for the true effect of height on EA, respectively. Thus, if the only source of endogeneity is pleiotropy, the results in Table 3 would suggest that the true standardized effect is between 0.11 (from GIV-U) and 0.17 (from GIV-C).
However, the negative control regression results in Table 4 provide substantial evidence of endogeneity bias from environmental sources. Given that the true effect of EA on height should be zero, then GIV-U would accurately estimate this effect to be zero in the absence of environmental endogeneity. Instead, GIV-U reports a significant positive effect of EA on height. MR also reports a positive and statistically significant effect of EA on height. This upward bias in the MR estimate is strong evidence of pleiotropy bias that invalidates the IV in MR. The guidance from the simulations points to a true estimate of the effect of height on EA that is as small or smaller than the GIV-U estimate, which is 25% smaller than the estimate from MR. The extent of upward bias in the GIV-U estimate depends on the strength of environmental variables that simultaneously affected the height of HRS respondents and also affected their EA.
Table 4.
Variable | OLS | MR | GIV-C | GIV-U |
EA | 0.072*** | 0.179** | 0.050*** | 0.040*** |
(0.0138) | (0.0543) | (0.0119) | (0.0120) | |
PGS height cond. UKB | 0.448*** | |||
(0.0174) | ||||
PGS height unc. UKB | 0.446*** | |||
(0.0175) | ||||
2,751 | 2,751 | 2,751 | 2,751 |
*, **, ***. Standardized effect sizes and SEs are in parentheses. Birth year, birth year squared, gender, EA mother, EA father, and the first 20 principal components are included as control variables. For MR, a PGS for EA from UK Biobank (UKB) data was used as an instrument for EA. For GIV-C and GIV-U PGS height GIANT was used as an instrument.
These results also point toward a productive strategy for learning more about the true effect of height on EA. The demonstration that pleiotropy as well as environmental bias is affecting the estimates in Table 3 implies that there is no effective fix for MR; its genetic instruments are contaminated by pleiotropy and therefore cannot be used to adjust for environmental endogeneity. Comparisons between monozygotic twins would effectively control for pleiotropy, but unobserved environmental factors affecting height and EA could still bias the results from MZ twin data. The most productive strategy is arguably to use GIV-U and GIV-C as proxy strategies to address bias from pleiotropy while also correcting for environmental confounds either by controlling for the relevant environmental factors directly or by using data on siblings that allow controlling for shared environmental variables via family fixed-effects. With such data, the estimates from GIV-C and GIV-U would provide approximate bounds as long as the GIV-U estimate of the effect of EA on height was close to zero. If the negative control regression suggests bias from environmental sources, the GIV-U estimate will be more conservative than all of the other estimators considered in this paper, and it will underpredict the true effect unless positive bias from both pleiotropy and genetic-unrelated endogeneity is quite strong.
These results do not provide as clean and neat a conclusion as might be desired, although uncertainty is inevitable in the absence of experimental data or a valid IV. At the same time, the empirical example provides considerable insight into the implications of the available estimates. Our results strongly imply that OLS provides an upwardly biased estimate of the effect of height on EA. They also strongly imply that the MR estimate suffers from pleiotropy bias and that MR is not an effective strategy for determining whether and to what extent height affects EA. Other studies suggest that pleiotropy between EA and height is not extremely high (18). Our simulation results therefore suggest that GIV-U either is a plausible estimate for the effect of height on EA if genetic-unrelated endogeneity is relatively strong or underpredicts this effect if the endogeneity is weaker. If the estimate of GIV-U continued to be positive and statistically significant in a sibling fixed-effects analysis, we would be rather confident that the effect is real and not an artifact of bias either from pleiotropy or from genetic-unrelated endogeneity. In other words, these results represent progress toward the goal of understanding how large the social advantage provided by height is in the process of EA given the existence of genetic confounds. (Obviously, none of the estimation strategies we discussed here address possible bias from nonrandom selection into samples.)
Conclusion
Accurate estimation of causal relationships with observational data is one of the biggest and most important challenges in epidemiology and the social sciences—two fields of inquiry where many questions of interest cannot be adequately addressed with properly designed experiments due to practical or ethical constraints. One important confound in nonexperimental data comes from direct pleiotropic effects of genes on the exposure and the outcome of interest. Both OLS and MR yield biased results in this case. We proposed GIV regression as an empirical strategy that controls for such pleiotropic effects using polygenic scores. GIV regression uses standard IV estimation algorithms such as two-stage least squares that are widely available in existing statistical software packages. Our approach provides reasonable upper and lower bounds of causal effects in situations when pleiotropic effects of genes are the only source of bias. We showed that OLS, MR, and GIV regression yield biased estimates if both genetic and environmental sources of endogeneity are present. However, GIV regression still outperforms OLS and MR in this scenario. Furthermore, GIV regression can (and should) be combined with additional strategies that allow controlling for bias from purely environmental or behavioral factors, such as using covariates or family-fixed effects. Together, these approaches can provide reasonable estimates of causal effects across a broad range of scenarios.
GIV regression is called for whenever an experimental design, a valid IV strategy, or a large-enough sample of MZ twins is not available and when pleiotropy is a potential problem—a situation that is frequently encountered in practice. The main requirements for GIV regression are a prediction sample that has been comprehensively genotyped and large-scale GWAS results for the outcome of interest from two nonoverlapping samples. Due to rapidly falling genotyping costs that enable a growing availability of genetic data and large GWAS samples for many traits, these requirements have become increasingly feasible for many applications. Indeed, the combination of new estimation tools and continued rapid advancements in genetics should provide a significant improvement in our understanding of the effects of behavioral and environmental variables on important socioeconomic and medical outcomes.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We are very grateful to Richard Karlsson Linnér for help with the GWAS analyses in the UK Biobank, to Aysu Okbay for providing us with subsets of the GWAS meta-analysis on educational attainment, and to S. Fleur Meddens for constructing genetic principal components in the HRS data. We thank Daniel J. Benjamin, Jonathan P. Beauchamp, Benjamin Domingue, Lisbeth Trille Loft, Niels Rietveld, Eric Slob, Patrick Turley, Hans van Kippersluis, and Tian Zheng for productive discussions and comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. Furthermore, we thank Elliot Tucker-Drob for directing us to the necessary correction of the heritability estimate in our model. This research was facilitated by the SSGAC and by the research group on genetic and social causes of life chances at the Zentrum für interdisciplinäre Forschung Bielefeld. Data analyses made use of the UK Biobank resource under Application 11425. We acknowledge data access from the GIANT Consortium. We used data from the HRS, which is supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA U01AG009740, RC2 AG036495, and RC4 AG039029). This study was supported by funding from a European Research Council Consolidator Grant (647648 EdGe) (to P.D.K.). HRS genotype data can be accessed via the database of Genotypes and Phenotypes (dbGaP, accession no. phs000428.v1.p1). Researchers who wish to link genetic data with other HRS measures that are not in dbGaP, such as educational attainment, must apply for access from HRS.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
See Commentary on page 5626.
*Fixed-effects estimation with panel data would also preclude MR-type strategies because the IV does not vary over time, and genetic indicators for would generally have a weak relationship to changes in over time. Fixed-effects regressions based on other strategies (e.g., sibling or neighborhood fixed-effects models) would not control for pleiotropy. We discuss these strategies at greater length below.
†An earlier version of our paper pursued this approach and called it GIV regression. However, we later found that controlling for in a GWAS for induces a correlation of with that invalidates the latter as an IV. The version of GIV-U and GIV-C regression we describe below does not have this problem because it does not use a genetic instrument for anymore. Instead, GIV-U and GIV-C both rely on a proxy-control strategy that uses only an instrument for or to correct for measurement error in these proxies for and .
‡It is possible to have finite-sample bias that disappears asymptotically, in which case the estimator is consistent. We use the expectation formula instead because it is arguably more straightforward to understand.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1707388115/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.McNeill PM. The Ethics and Politics of Human Experimentation. Cambridge Univ Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Stigler SM. Correlation and causation: A comment. Perspect Biol Med. 2005;48:S88–S94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lawlor DA, Smith GD, Ebrahim S. Commentary: The hormone replacement–coronary heart disease conundrum: Is this the death of observational epidemiology? Int J Epidemiol. 2004;33:464–467. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Plomin R. Genetics and general cognitive ability. Nature. 1999;402:C25–C29. doi: 10.1038/35011520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Yang J, et al. Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nat Genet. 2010;42:565–569. doi: 10.1038/ng.608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Yang J, Lee SH, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. Gcta: A tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis. Am J Hum Genet. 2011;88:76–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D’Onofrio B, Gottesman II. Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:623–628. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Polderman TJC, et al. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat Genet. 2015;47:702–709. doi: 10.1038/ng.3285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Conley D. Socio-genomic research using genome-wide molecular data. Annu Rev Sociol. 2016;42:275–299. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Chabris CF, Lee JJ, Cesarini D, Benjamin DJ, Laibson DI. The fourth law of behavior genetics. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2015;24:304–312. doi: 10.1177/0963721415580430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Paaby AB, Rockman MV. The many faces of pleiotropy. Trends Genet. 2013;29:66–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2012.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Solovieff N, Cotsapas C, Lee PH, Purcell SM, Smoller JW. Pleiotropy in complex traits: Challenges and strategies. Nat Rev Genet. 2013;14:483–495. doi: 10.1038/nrg3461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Low KB. In: Pleiotropy in Encyclopedia of Genetics. Brenner S, Miller JH, editors. Academic; New York: 2001. pp. 1490–1491. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Boyle EA, Li YI, Pritchard JK. An expanded view of complex traits: From polygenic to omnigenic. Cell. 2017;169:1177–1186. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Okbay A, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature. 2016;533:539–542. doi: 10.1038/nature17671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lynch M, Walsh B. Genetics and Analysis of Quantitative Traits. Sinauer; Sunderland, MA: 1998. Correlations between characters; pp. 629–656. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lee SH, Yang J, Goddard ME, Visscher PM, Wray NR. Estimation of pleiotropy between complex diseases using SNP-derived genomic relationships and restricted maximum likelihood. Bioinformatics. 2012;28:2540–2542. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bts474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bulik-Sullivan B, et al. An atlas of genetic correlations across human diseases and traits. Nat Genet. 2015;47:1236–1241. doi: 10.1038/ng.3406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Zheng J, et al. LD Hub: A centralized database and web interface to perform LD score regression that maximizes the potential of summary level GWAS data for SNP heritability and genetic correlation analysis. Bioinformatics. 2017;33:272–279. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btw613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.D’Onofrio BM, et al. A quasi-experimental study of maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring academic achievement. Child Dev. 2010;81:80–100. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01382.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Asbury K, Dunn JF, Pike A, Plomin R. Nonshared environmental influences on individual differences in early behavioral development: A monozygotic twin differences study. Child Dev. 2003;74:933–943. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Caspi A, et al. Maternal expressed emotion predicts children’s antisocial behavior problems: Using monozygotic-twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral development. Dev Psychol. 2004;40:149–161. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ehli EA, et al. De novo and inherited CNVs in MZ twin pairs selected for discordance and concordance on attention problems. Eur J Hum Genet. 2012;20:1037–1043. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2012.49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wooldridge JM. Econometric Analysis of Cross Section and Panel Data. Massachusetts Institute of Technology; Cambridge, MA: 2002. pp. 83–113. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Smith GD, Ebrahim S. ‘Mendelian randomization’: Can genetic epidemiology contribute to understanding environmental determinants of disease? Int J Epidemiol. 2003;32:1–22. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyg070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Davey Smith G, Hemani G. Mendelian randomization: Genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Hum Mol Genet. 2014;23:R89–R98. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddu328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Pickrell J. 2015. Fulfilling the promise of Mendelian randomization. bioRxiv:018150.
- 28.Davey Smith G. 2015. Mendelian randomization: A premature burial? bioRxiv:021386.
- 29.Hahn J, Hausman J. Weak instruments: Diagnosis and cures in empirical econometrics. Am Econ Rev. 2003;93:118–125. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Murray MP. Avoiding invalid instruments and coping with weak instruments. J Econ Perspect. 2006;20:111–132. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hamer D, Sirota L. Beware the chopsticks gene. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5:11–13. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bowden J, Davey Smith G, Burgess S. Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: Effect estimation and bias detection through Egger regression. Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44:512–525. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Verbanck M, Chen CY, Neale B, Do R. 2017. Widespread pleiotropy confounds causal relationships between complex traits and diseases inferred from Mendelian randomization. bioRxiv:157552.
- 34.Welter D, et al. The NHGRI GWAS Catalog, a curated resource of SNP-trait associations. Nucleic Acids Res. 2014;42:D1001–D1006. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkt1229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Wood AR, et al. Defining the role of common variation in the genomic and biological architecture of adult human height. Nat Genet. 2014;46:1173–1186. doi: 10.1038/ng.3097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Locke AE, et al. Genetic studies of body mass index yield new insights for obesity biology. Nature. 2015;518:197–206. doi: 10.1038/nature14177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ripke S, et al. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511:421–427. doi: 10.1038/nature13595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lambert JC, et al. Meta-analysis of 74,046 individuals identifies 11 new susceptibility loci for Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Genet. 2013;45:1452–1458. doi: 10.1038/ng.2802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Okbay A, et al. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nat Genet. 2016;29:1591. doi: 10.1038/ng1216-1587b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Price AL, et al. Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet. 2006;38:904–909. doi: 10.1038/ng1847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Rietveld CA, et al. Replicability and robustness of GWAS for behavioral traits. Psychol Sci. 2014;25:1975–1986. doi: 10.1177/0956797614545132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bulik-Sullivan BK, et al. LD score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet. 2015;47:291–295. doi: 10.1038/ng.3211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Loh PR, et al. Efficient Bayesian mixed-model analysis increases association power in large cohorts. Nat Genet. 2015;47:284–290. doi: 10.1038/ng.3190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Purcell SM, et al. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature. 2009;460:748–752. doi: 10.1038/nature08185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Dudbridge F. Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genet. 2013;9:e1003348. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Rietveld CA, et al. GWAS of 126,559 individuals identifies genetic variants associated with educational attainment. Science. 2013;340:1467–1471. doi: 10.1126/science.1235488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Witte JS, Visscher PM, Wray NR. The contribution of genetic variants to disease depends on the ruler. Nat Rev Genet. 2014;15:765–776. doi: 10.1038/nrg3786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bowden J, Davey Smith G, Burgess S. Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: Effect estimation and bias detection through Egger regression. Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44:512–525. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.van Kippersluis H, Rietveld CA. Pleiotropy-robust Mendelian randomization. Int J Epidemiol. 2017 doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Zhu Z, et al. Causal associations between risk factors and common diseases inferred from GWAS summary data. Nat Commun. 2018;9:224. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02317-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Sonnega A, et al. Cohort profile: The Health and Retirement Study (HRS) Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43:576–585. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Daetwyler HD, Villanueva B, Woolliams JA. Accuracy of predicting the genetic risk of disease using a genome-wide approach. PLoS One. 2008;3:e3395. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bielby WT, Hauser RM, Featherman DL. Response errors of nonblack males in models of the stratification process. J Am Stat Assoc. 1977;72:723–735. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Bollen KA. Latent variables in psychology and the social sciences. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:605–634. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Angrist JD, Pischke JS. Mostly Harmless Econometrics: An Empiricist’s Companion. Princeton Univ Press; Princeton: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Wickens MR. A note on the use of proxy variables. Econometrica. 1972;40:759–761. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Aigner DJ. MSE dominance of least squares with errors-of-observation. J Econom. 1974;2:365–372. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Pagan A. Econometric issues in the analysis of regressions with generated regressors. Int Econ Rev. 1984;25:221–247. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Murphy KM, Topel RH. Estimation and inference in two-step econometric models. J Bus Econ Stat. 2002;20:88–97. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Burgess S, Small DS, Thompson SG. A review of instrumental variable estimators for Mendelian randomization. Stat Meth Med Res. 2015;26:2333–2355. doi: 10.1177/0962280215597579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Tucker-Drob EM. 2017. Measurement error correction of genome-wide polygenic scores in prediction samples. bioRxiv:165472.
- 62.Burgess S, Butterworth A, Malarstig A, Thompson SG. Use of Mendelian randomisation to assess potential benefit of clinical intervention. BMJ. 2012;345:e7325. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e7325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Kong A, et al. 2017. The nature of nurture: Effects of parental genotypes. bioRxiv:219261.
- 64.de Vlaming R, et al. Meta-GWAS Accuracy and Power (MetaGAP) calculator shows that hiding heritability is partially due to imperfect genetic correlations across studies. PLoS Genetics. 2017;13 doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006495. e1006495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Yang J, et al. Genetic variance estimation with imputed variants finds negligible missing heritability for human height and body mass index. Nat Genet. 2015;47:1114–1120. doi: 10.1038/ng.3390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Silventoinen K, Kaprio J, Lahelma E. Genetic and environmental contributions to the association between body height and educational attainment: A study of adult Finnish twins. Behav Genet. 2000;30:477–485. doi: 10.1023/a:1010202902159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Case A, Paxson C, Islam M. Making sense of the labor market height premium: Evidence from the British household panel survey. Econ Lett. 2009;102:174–176. doi: 10.1016/j.econlet.2008.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Case A, Paxson C. Stature and status: Height, ability, and labor market outcomes. J Polit Econ. 2008;116:499–532. doi: 10.1086/589524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Heineck G. Up in the skies? The relationship between body height and earnings in Germany. Labour. 2005;19:469–489. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Heineck G. Too tall to be smart? The relationship between height and cognitive abilities. Econ Lett. 2009;105:78–80. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Schick A, Steckel RH. Height, human capital, and earnings: The contributions of cognitive and noncognitive ability. J Hum Cap. 2015;9:94–115. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Weitzman A, Conley D. 2014. From assortative to ashortative coupling: Men’s height, height heterogamy, and relationship dynamics in the United States (National Bureau of Economic Research), Technical Report.
- 73.Kanazawa S. Big and tall parents have more sons: Further generalizations of the Trivers–Willard hypothesis. J Theor Biol. 2005;235:583–590. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Cinnirella F, Piopiunik M, Winter J. Why does height matter for educational attainment? Evidence from German children. Econ Hum Biol. 2011;9:407–418. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2011.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Marchini J, et al. 2015 Genotype imputation and genetic association studies of UK biobank: Interim data release. Available at http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/wp-content/uploads/2014/04/imputation_documentation_May2015.pdf. Accessed April 18, 2017.
- 76.Lee JJ, et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a 1.1-million-person GWAS of educational attainment. Nat Genet. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.