Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Dec 13.
Published in final edited form as: Cell. 2018 Dec 13;175(7):1827–1841.e17. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.11.032

Multisensory logic of infant-directed aggression by males

Yoh Isogai 1,2,*, Zheng Wu 1,4, Michael I Love 3,5, Michael Ho-Young Ahn 1, Dhananjay Bambah-Mukku 1, Vivian Hua 1, Karolina Farrell 2, Catherine Dulac 1,6,*
PMCID: PMC6558521  NIHMSID: NIHMS1515902  PMID: 30550786

Summary

Newborn mice emit signals that promote parenting from mothers and fathers, but trigger aggressive responses from virgin males. Although pup-directed attacks by males require vomeronasal function, the specific infant cues that elicit this behavior are unknown. We developed a behavioral paradigm based on reconstituted pup cues and showed that discrete infant morphological features combined with salivary chemosignals elicit robust male aggression. Seven vomeronasal receptors were identified based on infant-mediated activity, and the involvement of two receptors, Vmn2r65, and Vmn2r88, in infant-directed aggression was demonstrated by genetic deletion. Using the activation of these receptors as readouts for biochemical fractionation, we isolated two pheromonal compounds, the submandibular gland protein C and hemoglobins. Unexpectedly, none of the identified vomeronasal receptors and associated cues were specific to pups. Thus, infant-mediated aggression by virgin males relies on the recognition of pup’s physical traits in addition to parental and infant chemical cues.

Graphical Abstract

graphic file with name nihms-1515902-f0001.jpg

Introduction

In natural environments, animals receive a barrage of sensory information from which salient features trigger behavioral actions (Tinbergen, 1969; Stowers et al., 2002). In turn, sensory information and behavioral decisions are gated by the animal’s internal state, enabling sensory inputs to generate motor outputs that are adapted to a given context (Beach, 1942a; Marder, 2012; Chen and Hong, 2018).

The behavioral responses of adult mice to infant cues provide a dramatic example of behavioral variation according to the animal’s internal state. When presented with pups, virgin females, mothers and fathers exhibit parental care, which includes nest building, grooming, crouching over pups, aggression towards intruders, and nursing in females. By contrast, virgin males display infanticidal behavior, in which they attack and kill infants (McCarthy and vom Saal, 1985). Previous studies have demonstrated the critical roles of olfactory, somatosensory and auditory cues in maternal behavior (Beach and Jaynes, 1956; Stern, 1990; Marlin et al., 2015). By contrast, vomeronasal function appears largely dispensable for parenting (Kimchi et al., 2007; Tachikawa et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2014). In virgin males, however, genetic and surgical inactivation of the vomeronasal organ (VNO) leads to the loss of pup-directed aggressive behavior, and to the robust expression of parental care (Mennella and Moltz, 1988; Tachikawa et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2014), demonstrating the essential role of VNO cues in eliciting infanticide and in repressing parenting behavior. However, the identity of infant cues that trigger male attacks and the specific vomeronasal receptors (VRs) that detect these cues remain uncharacterized.

To uncover the nature of pup signals leading to aggressive responses in males, we developed a behavioral paradigm in which mice were exposed to reconstituted chemical and physical features of a mouse infant. Remarkably, in addition to pup chemosignals, discrete morphological traits emerged as an essential trigger to infant-directed aggression. To further dissect the molecular basis of VNO-mediated infant recognition, we identified the entire repertoire of 7 VNO receptors (VRs) detecting pup cues, and exploited these findings to purify two new pheromonal compounds, the submandibular gland protein C, and hemoglobin beta, which activate the Vmn2r65 receptor and the Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r122/123 receptors, respectively. Single and double knockout of Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 demonstrated their role in male infanticide. Unexpectedly, neither the pup-associated cues nor the receptors responsive to these cues were pup-specific, and instead were shared with adult conspecific cues and their receptors. Altogether, our data suggest that infant-mediated attack by virgin males relies on the detection of multiple pheromonal compounds of broad specificity together with distinctive infant morphological traits.

The surprising lack of VRs and corresponding chemosignals unique to pup contrasts with the large number and high specificity of VRs sensing adult mouse and predator cues and suggests that integrative circuit mechanisms rather than labeled line pathways may drive stereotyped instinctive behaviors such as infanticide by males. Moreover, this vomeronasal “stealth” may point to the conflicting needs of infants to be both concealed and advertised to adults in order to survive and be nurtured.

Results

Reconstitution of signals for pup-directed aggression

Virgin males attack dead pups vigorously, indicating that vocalization, body temperature and movement are not essential cues for pup-directed aggression (Figure 1A, B). Further, pup-directed male aggression occurs in full darkness, suggesting that vision is also dispensable (not shown). This prompted us to design silicone pup dummies recapitulating the morphological traits of a mouse pup, such as its size, shape, and texture (Figure 1A).

Figure 1: Combination of physical and chemical signals triggers pup-directed aggression.

Figure 1:

(A) Images of real pup and four silicone pup decoys used in these experiments

(B) Raster plots of behaviors by virgin males (N = 19) presented with dummies swabbed with C57BL/6J salivary extract (top) or dead C57BL/6J pups (bottom). Red: aggressive encounters with biting, blue: chemo-investigative bouts.

(C) Total aggression per 5 min assay is significantly longer when males are exposed to dead pups compared to dummies (p < 0.001 by t-Test). N = 19.

(D) Latency to attack is significantly shorter when males are exposed to dead pups compared to dummies (p < 0.01 by t-Test). N = 19.

(E,F) Durations of aggressive interactions and all contacts in the initial 3 min of encounters in which males are presented with dummies swabbed with PBS (blue), pup salivary extracts (red) and adult male urine (yellow). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 by paired t-Test, N = 15. Connected circles represent data obtained with the same males.

(G) Sliding window analysis of aggressive interactions between males and dummies swabbed with various chemical cues. Top graph shows the cumulative duration of aggressive bouts in 2 min sliding windows. Bottom graph shows the p values of the paired t-Test for differences in aggressive bout durations between dummies swabbed with PBS and pup salivary extracts (blue) or between dummies swabbed with male urine and pup salivary extracts (brown) in 2 min sliding windows. N = 15.

(H) Raster plots of behaviors by virgin males exposed to various silicone dummies. Each event marks an interaction between males and the introduced pup decoy swabbed with pup salivary extract, with events categorized as aggressive (red) or non-aggressive (blue). N = 15.

(I) p-values of the paired t-Test for the differences in aggressive bout durations between decoys of various shapes swabbed with PBS and pup salivary extracts (blue) or between decoys swabbed with male urine and pup salivary extracts (brown) tested in 2 min sliding windows. Note the late attack against the “hybrid” decoy. N = 15.

(J) Cumulative aggression times by virgin males presented with pup decoys of four different shapes swabbed with pup salivary extract *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001 by paired t-Test. N = 15.

Pup dummies conditioned for an hour with scents from a cage containing a mother and pups elicited robust attacking behavior (Supplemental video 1). Intriguingly, the majority of males attacking pup dummies displayed the stereotypical behaviors seen in aggressive episodes with live or dead pups, such as chemo-investigation, rough handling, aggressive grooming, and biting. We compared aggression directed against dead or dummy pups swabbed with pup salivary gland extracts and established that, dummies conditioned with pup scents (e.g., pup salivary extracts) provoked naturalistic pup-directed aggression (Figure 1B; Supplemental video 1,2), such that the initial investigation of pups by males switches to quickly alternating chemo-investigation and biting. Occasionally, males grab dummies by the mouth, and carry them within the cage with jerky movements, before repeatedly biting them on the ground (Supplemental video 1). Compared to dummies swabbed with pup scents, dead pups provoked more sustained attacks and shorter latency to the first attack (Figure 1BD), suggesting that additional cues emitted by dead pups intensify male aggression. However, the aggressive behavior of males toward silicone dummies conditioned with pup scents closely mimics attacks of real pups.

To further characterize the minimal set of stimuli required for pup-directed aggression, we swabbed dummies with pup salivary extracts as a source of pup cues; phosphate buffered saline (PBS) as negative control; and adult male urine, a source of chemosignals known to trigger male-directed attacks (Chamero et al., 2007).

Interestingly, dummies coated with PBS alone elicited some low level of attacks (Figure 1E). However, swabbing dummies with pup salivary extracts significantly increased the duration of aggressive bouts and chemoinvestigative contacts compared to PBS or adult male urine swabbed dummies (Figure 1F). The initial 3 min of the behavior assay contained the highest frequency of aggressive events, and salivary extracts led to significantly higher attack duration and contact time in this period compared to PBS or urine swabbed dummies (Figure 1G).

Collectively, these experiments demonstrated that signals required for pup-directed attacks can be reconstituted using inanimate cues. Importantly, specific chemical cues produced in pup salivary glands significantly enhanced pup-directed aggression, while cues known to be involved in adult-adult aggression, such as adult male urine, did not.

Specific morphological features elicit pup-directed aggression

Next we asked whether specific physical features were instrumental in triggering male attacks during interactions with pups.

Fast-frame videography of adult males investigating pups indicated that males made extensive orofacial contact with pups (Supplemental video 3), prompting us to examine the effect of infant morphological traits in dummy-directed aggression. We tested three additional dummy shapes: the brick, an unnatural object of similar size with sharp edges; the blob, a legless dummy with pup-like head and body curvature; and the hybrid, which combined the brick with limbs and a tail (Figure 1A). Exposure of males to a brick failed to induce aggression, even when painted with pup salivary extracts (p < 0.001, Student’s t-Test, N = 15; Figure 1HJ, Supplemental figure 1). Likewise, the blob did not elicit aggression (p = 0.12, Student’s t-Test, N = 15). Strikingly, however, the hybrid shape with hind and front legs and tail partially restored aggression (p < 0.01, Student’s t-Test, N = 15), although to a smaller extent than the more realistic dummy shape (p < 0.05, Student’s t-Test, N = 15; Figure 1J).

These results revealed that the identification of pups by aggressive males is a multi-sensory process, involving the recognition of specific physical and chemical signals.

A discrete set of seven VRs detect pup chemosignals

We next aimed to analyze in more detail the contribution of VNO detection in this behavior. To search for VRs activated by pups, we assessed the induction of the immediate early gene Egr1, a sensitive molecular readout of VNO neural activity (Isogai et al., 2011), and visualized the activation of subpopulations of VNO neurons following encounters of adult virgin males and females with C57BL6/J pups (Figure 2A,B).

Figure 2: Pup cues activate seven VRs in the adult VNO.

Figure 2:

(A,B) Egr1 induction in the VNO of virgin male and female mice upon pup exposure. The number of Egr1+ VNO neurons identified in these experiments is shown in (B). Each data point represents one animal. Number of animals used is indicated in the graph. ***p < 0.001 by t-Test. Dark lines indicate the mean, and light lines indicate the quartiles.

(C) Top four panels: double FISH with RNA probes to Egr1 and candidate VRs led to the identification of five VRs responding to pup cues. Arrows indicate cells in which Egr1 (green) and VR signals (red) co-localize. Bottom panel: double FISH with RNA probes to Egr1 (green) and a mix of the five VR probes shown in panels above. White arrows point to co-labeled neurons, and green arrows indicate Egr1+ neurons that express none of the 5 identified VRs.

(D) Immunofluorescence with anti-pS6 antibody and in situ hybridization with RNA probe to Egr1 demonstrates co-expression (arrows) in VNO neurons activated by pup cues (94.7 ± 2.2 % of Egr1+ neurons are pS6+; mean ± SEM; 699 Egr1+ cells from 3 animals examined).

(E) Experimental pipeline for immuno-purification of phospho-ribosome associated RNAs from VNOs of animals exposed to pup cues.

(F) MA (log2 difference over signal strength) plot showing the enrichment of specific VR in the VNOs of virgin males exposed to pup stimuli compared to fresh bedding. Red dots represent genes significantly (p < 0.1) over-represented in pup-exposed VNO samples. Green dots represent enriched VR genes.

(G) List of VRs identified in the pS6-based screen. The screen failed to identify the sparsely represented V1ri9 identified by the candidate VR screen (shown in C).

(H) Comprehensive set of VRs activated by pup cues. Double FISH with individual VR and Egr1 probes. Are shown the percentages of co-labeled neurons (N = 3 animals with total number of VR+ cells analyzed indicated in parentheses).

(I) Double RNA FISH with probes to Egr1 and the mix of 7 receptor probes shown in H (N = 3 animals).

(J) Summary of responses by VRs, identified based on their recognition of pup cues, to non-pup cues, from data by Isogai et al. 2011 and data shown in Supplemental figure 2A.

All scale bars represent 100 μm unless indicated otherwise.

Because pups are taken from the mother’s cage and transferred to the behavioral arena, we tested the potential contribution of maternal odor in our assays, and sampled bedding from cages in which mothers had been separated from their pups and housed alone for 2 days. This exposure resulted in the activation of about 4-fold fewer Egr1+neurons than from pup exposure (Figure 2A,B), suggesting that our assay robustly monitors VNO activation by pup cues, even in the context of mother-infant cohabitation.

Following exposure to pups, significantly fewer Egr1+ cells were detected in the VNO of virgin females and non-aggressive virgin males compared to aggressive virgin males (p < 0.001 by Student’s t-Test, virgin females N = 9, non-aggressive males N = 3; Figure 2B). This is consistent with a previous report showing that VNOs of fathers have reduced c-fos activation following pup exposure compared to virgin males (Tachikawa et al., 2013), and it extends this observation to males lacking aggressive display towards pups, irrespective of their status as virgin or fathers.

Next, we used an in situ hybridization-based screen to uncover the identity of VRs expressed in Egr1+ neurons following pup encounters by aggressive males (Star Methods; Isogai et al., 2011). Five VRs were identified: Vmn2r88 (representing 43.4 ± 0.8 % of Egr1+ cells; mean ± SEM), V1rc1 or V1rc30 (15.6 ± 1.5 %; our probe does not distinguish between these 2 closely related transcripts), Vmn2r65 (11.2 ± 1.6 %), and V1ri9 (9.0 ± 1.7 %) (Figure 2C,H).

Approximately 20 % of Egr1+ cells were not covered by the pool of five VR probes, suggesting that one or few additional VRs to pup cues may exist that were not identified in this candidate-based screen (Figure 2C bottom panel, green arrows). We therefore performed a second, unbiased, screen of transcripts associated with the phosphorylated ribosomal protein S6 (pS6) in activated neurons (Knight et al., 2012). Control experiments confirmed that the phosphorylation of the S6 is robustly induced in Egr1+ neurons upon pup exposure (Figure 2D). Next, we captured the transcriptome of neurons activated by pup cues or fresh bedding by immunopurification of phosphoribosome associated RNAs (Figure 2E). Data from RNA-seq of pS6 pulled down transcripts were analyzed and searched for transcripts enriched in VNOs from animals exposed to pup cues.

Among these transcripts were genes encoding VRs, but no genes encoding odorant receptors or other classes of known chemosensory receptors (Figure 2F), as well as V1rc1, V1rc30, Vmn2r65, and Vmn2r88, i.e., four out of five VR transcripts identified in our earlier screen (Figure 2G). Two closely related genes V1rc1 and V1rc30, which could not be distinguished from each other by in situ hybridization, encoded receptors that are both activated by pup cues. In addition, a VR sequence annotated as pseudogene Vmn2r-ps159 emerged from the pS6 screen. Upon closer examination, these reads matched perfectly with the sequences of Vmn2r122 and Vmn2r123, previously cloned but not mapped to the genome (Matsunami and Buck, 1997; Ryba and Tirindelli, 1997). We confirmed that VNO neurons expressing Vmn2r122 or 123 (denoted as Vmn2r122/123 since they could not be distinguished by in situ hybridization) were robustly activated by pup cues, accounting for 14.0 ± 2.2 % of Egr1+ neurons (Figure 2H). Overall, the expression of the seven VR genes identified here accounted for 92.2 ± 1.2 % of the VNO neurons activated by pups (Figure 2I), suggesting that we reached a nearly comprehensive identification of VRs to infant cues.

Surprisingly, published work as well as our data revealed that none of the identified VRs were specific to pup cues (Figure 2J), such that they were each also stimulated by either adult male or female cues (Figure 2J). Thus, none of the receptors activated by pup cues conveyed specific information about infants – although they may do so as a group.

Identifying the sources of infant chemosignals

Next, we sought to exploit the identification of VRs activated by pups to search for the corresponding molecular cues. Over 50 % of VNO neurons activated by pups express V2Rs, among which the majority express either Vmn2r65 or Vmn2r88, prompting us to focus on the specific compounds recognized by these two VRs.

Since Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 were robustly activated by both adult and pups cues, we first tested the ability of tissue homogenates from organs previously implicated in pheromone production and release, i.e., adult salivary and lacrimal glands (Figure 3A), as well as that of urine, to stimulate Vmn2r65- and Vmn2r88-expressing VNO neurons.

Figure 3: The origins of pup stimuli.

Figure 3:

(A) Schematic diagram depicting the location of various exocrine glands (green: lacrimal, orange: salivary, pink: preputial) used to test Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 ligand activity.

(B) Summary of VR activity using gland extracts from adults and pups, and adult urine. n/d: not determined.

(C) Vmn2r65 ligand activity is found exclusively in female submaxillary gland extract. RNA FISH with Egr1 and Vmn2r65 probes on virgin male VNOs after exposure to urine and gland extracts from adult virgin females. Arrows mark Egr1 and Vmn2r65 co-expression.

(D) Vmn2r88 ligand activity is found in extracts of multiple glands. RNA FISH with Egr1 and Vmn2r88 probes on VNOs from virgin males after exposure to urine and gland extracts from adult virgin males. Arrows mark Egr1 and Vmn2r88 co-expression.

(E-F) Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r65 ligand activities are found in both pup and adult tissue extracts. All experiments were performed on 2 animals per stimulus, and the mean percentage of VR+Egr1+ neurons in VR+ cells in virgin male VNOs is indicated, along with the number of cells analyzed. All errors are in SEM.

All scale bars represent 100 μm.

Interestingly, Vmn2r65-expressing neurons were exclusively stimulated by extracts of adult female submaxillary and sublingual glands but not by extracts from any other tissues, or by female urine (Figure 3B,C). Moreover, this activity was exclusively present in female but not male tissues (Figure 3F). In contrast, the ligand activity for Vmn2r88 was detected in extracts from multiple tissues in both males and females, most strongly in salivary and lacrimal glands (Figure 3B,D,F). Salivary extracts from C57BL/6J pup also activated Vmn2r65- and Vmn2r88-expressing neurons (Figure 3B,E). In addition, salivary extracts from both male and female pups from four distinct mouse strains stimulated Vmn2r65, Vmn2r88, and Vmn2r122/123-expressing neurons (Supplemental figure 3).

Thus, both adults and pups produce ligands for Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88, further strengthening our initial observation that receptors identified based on their activation by pup cues also recognize adult cues. Moreover, our data suggest that responses of Vmn2r65, 88, and Vmn2r122/123 neurons to pup cues are neither sex nor strain specific.

Biochemical purification of compounds associated with pup recognition

We performed biochemical fractionations of tissue homogenates from adult C57BL/6J salivary glands to isolate compounds stimulating Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 expressing neurons (Figure 4A, 5A). First, we used size exclusion chromatography to separate molecules by size, then exposed animals to individual fractions and quantified the percentage of activated (Egr1+) cells among Vmn2r65- or Vmn2r88-expressing VNO neurons. Our initial analysis revealed that ligands for Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 were likely macromolecules with molecular weight of 60–80 kDa and 30–40 kDa, respectively (Figure 4B; Figure 5B). We further purified these samples by ion exchange chromatography (Figure 4C,D; Figure 5D,E), and analyzed active fractions by mass spectrometry (MS, Methods).

Figure 4: The submandibular gland protein C (Smgc) activates neurons expressing Vmn2r65.

Figure 4:

(A) Purification strategy of compound(s) activating Vmn2r65 neurons (see Methods). The mass spectrometry profile of the peak activity fractions obtained from adult female salivary gland extracts was compared to the most homologous fractions obtained from male gland extracts.

(B) Activity profile of gel filtration chromatography fractions using female C57BL/6J salivary gland extract as a starting material. The ligand activity for Vmn2r65 was assayed by exposure of virgin males to selected fractions. Each dot represents the mean percentage of Vmn2r65+Egr1+ neurons among all Vmn2r65+ neurons from quantified on at least four VNO sections. Molecular weight for each fraction was estimated by SDS-PAGE. The ligand activity appears to elute broadly from this column, spanning 60 to 90 kDa.

(C) Activity profile of anion exchange chromatography fractions using Sepharose DEAE peak activity fractions as the input. The arrow marks the peak activity fraction (assayed in D), which was further analyzed by mass spectrometry.

(D) RNA FISH showing that Vmn2r65 neurons are activated by the exposure of virgin males to the peak fraction of anion exchange chromatography.

(E) SDS-PAGE gel of recombinant histidine-FLAG double-tagged Smgc produced in E. coli.

(F) Exposure of virgin males to recombinant Smgc results in specific activation of Vmn2r65+ VNO neurons. RNA FISH of VNO sections from virgin males exposed to recombinant Smgc shows co-localization of Egr1 and Vmn2r65 (arrows) (84.2 ± 5.0% of Vmn2r65+ neurons; Error in SEM; 166 Vmn2r65+ neurons from 3 animals examined).

(G) Fractionated C57BL6/J pup salivary gland extracts activate Vmn2r65-expressing neurons.

All scale bars represent 100 μm.

Figure 5: Hemoglobins are polymorphic ligands of Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r122/123.

Figure 5:

(A) Purification scheme of the Vmn2r88 ligand (see Methods).

(B) Activity profile of C57BL6/J male salivary gland extracts after gel filtration chromatography. The activity was assessed as the percentage of Vmn2r88-expressing neurons with Egr1 signal.

(C) Adult C57BL/6J blood activates Vmn2r88-expressing VNO neurons (Male blood: 92.5 ± 1.8 % of Vmn2r88 neurons; Error in SEM; 161 Vmn2r88 neurons from 2 animals; Female blood: 97.3 ± 0.4 % of Vmn2r88 neurons; 152 Vmn2r88 neurons from 2 animals examined).

(D) Vmn2r88-stimulating activity of the purified Poros HQ peak fraction confirmed by RNA FISH. Arrows mark colocalization between Egr1 and Vmn2r88.

(E) Protein gel of the Poros HQ active peak fraction used for mass spectrometry.

(F) Recombinant hemoglobin produced in E. coli is red, indicating the presence of heme.

(G) Recombinant Hbb-bt/Hba-a1 robustly activates Vmn2r88-expressing neurons (94.1 ± 0.9 % of Vmn2r88 neurons; mean ± SEM; 240 Vmn2r88 neurons from 4 animals examined) and Vmn2r122/123-expressing neurons (63.4 ± 16.4 % of Vmn2r122/123 neurons; 184 Vmn2r122/123 neurons, 3 animals). Virgin males were exposed to recombinant Hbs and their VNOs were analyzed for the activation of Egr1 in Vmn2r88 or Vmn2r122/123 neurons. (H,I) Specificity of Vmn2r88 activation by Hbs. Virgin males were exposed to recombinant Hbb-b1/Hba-a1 and Hbb-b2/Hba-a1, and their VNOs were analyzed by RNA FISH with Egr1 and Vmn2r88 probes. The intensities of the Egr1 signals in Vmn2r88 neurons in Figure 5G,H, represented as fold increase from the standard deviation of image background, are plotted in a graph in Figure 5I. Each dot represents one cell. ***p < 0.001 by t-Test.

(J) Vmn2r88 ligand activity is deposited in bedding of males and in bedding of mothers cohabitating with infants. Mother/pup bedding robustly excites Vmn2r88 neurons, as evidenced by RNA FISH showing the colocalization of Egr1 and Vmn2r88 signals in the VNO of animals exposed to these cues (arrows). All experiments used 3 or 4 animals, and the percentage of Vmn2r88+Egr1+ cells in Vmn2r88+ neurons are indicated, along with the total number of cells analyzed. All errors are in SEM.

(K) Plot of Egr1 signals (represented as fold increase from the standard deviation of image background) in Vmn2r88 neurons, stimulated by bedding of cross-fostered cages. Bedding from cross-fostered cages (one 129S1/SvImJ mother with C57BL/6J pups, one 129S1/SvImJ mother with 129S1/SvImJ pups, one C57BL/6J mother with 129S1/SvImJ pups, N = 3 each). **p < 0.01 by Student’s t-Test.

(L) Hemoglobin is readily detectable in the cage of pregnant and post-partum females with pups. Shown is a protein blot of bedding extracts probed by anti-Hb beta antibody. Bedding from three females (A, B, C) for each condition and males (D,E,F) are used for this panel.

(M,N) Recombinant Hbb-bt/Hba-a1 was stored at room temperature for indicated period of time and tested for its ability to stimulate Vmn2r88-expressing neurons. Two CD-1 virgin males were used for this test per sample. The box plot in (N) shows the quantification of the Vmn2r88 neurons co-labeled with Egr1. 4 sections from 2 animals were quantified for each time point.

All scale bars represent 100 μm unless otherwise indicated.

This strategy provided us with short lists of candidate ligands for Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88, which were further narrowed down based on our earlier identification of the strain-, sex-, and salivary gland specificity of ligand activities (Supplemental figure 4A, 5A). Control homogenates from different tissues, strains (C57B6/J and 129S1/SvImJ) and sexes were subjected to identical purification procedures. Specifically, fractionation from male salivary glands was used as a negative control for the Vmn2r65-stimulating activity, which we had earlier identified in gland extracts from pups and adult females only. Likewise, salivary gland extracts from adult males of the 129S1/SvImJ strain had no or only very weak activity on neurons expressing Vmn2r88 and thus served as a negative control for this activity. As further described below, the combination of Egr1 induction to monitor ligand activity on specific VNO neurons, together with our MS-based screening provided a powerful and efficient platform to identify novel pheromone ligands.

Vmn2r65 is activated by the submandibular gland protein C (Smgc)

Based on previously identified ligands of the large metabotropic glutamate receptor subfamily, we assumed that Vmn2r65 ligands would likely consist of polypeptides (Liberles, 2014). Moreover, the protein(s) of interest should be secreted in order to act as pheromone(s).

The highly purified, active fraction from female salivary glands was strikingly enriched in submandibular gland protein C (Smgc), a large 70 kDa protein previously identified from rat and mouse salivary glands (Zinzen et al., 2004) (Figure 4C,D; Supplemental figure 4A). Interestingly, Smgc is expressed by juveniles of both sexes but exclusively by adult females (Zinzen et al., 2004), consistent with the specificity of activation of Vmn2r65 by juvenile and adult tissue extracts shown earlier (Figure 3E,F; Supplemental figure 3).

We produced full-length recombinant Smgc in E. coli and confirmed its biological activity on Vmn2r65-expressing neurons in virgin males, thus demonstrating Smgc as a ligand for Vmn2r65 (Figure 4E,F). Importantly, Smgc was also identified by MS of partially fractionated pup salivary glands (Figure 4G; Supplemental figure 4B), confirming that adult females and pups commonly produce Smgc as a pheromone.

Vmn2r88 is activated by specific paralogs of hemoglobin

We next compared MS datasets obtained with active fractions of salivary gland extracts from C57BL/6J males and females with equivalent (but largely inactive) fractions obtained from 129S1/SvImJ mice. This strategy resulted in a list of secreted proteins that are candidate Vmn2r88 ligands (Supplemental figure 5A). We also noticed that the active purified fractions had a red tint (Supplemental figure 5B). Intriguingly, MS identified hemoglobins (Hbs) among the top candidate Vmn2r88 ligands (Supplemental figure 5A), and the Vmn2r88-stimulating activity was indeed present in blood lysate (Figure 5C). The purified active fraction from salivary gland extracts visualized on a SDS-PAGE gel showed prominent 14 kDa bands (Figure 5D,E), which matches the molecular weight of the monomeric Hb subunits alpha and beta.

Beta subunits of mouse Hb are strain specific (Weaver et al., 1981), and Hbb-bt (also known as Hbbt1; Supplemental figure 5A), the C57BL/6J-specific Hb beta subunit identified by MS satisfied the expected strain-specificity of the Vmn2r88 ligand activity. To reconstitute Hb complexes occurring in vivo, we co-expressed recombinant alpha and beta subunits in E. coli and purified the resulting complexes (Figure 5F), the vast majority of which proved to be heterodimers of Hbb-bt and Hba-a1 with a minor contribution of tetramers (Supplemental figure 5F,G). We then presented the reconstituted Hb complexes to virgin male mice in order to test if they displayed Vmn2r88 ligand activity.

Indeed, VNO neurons expressing Vmn2r88 showed robustly activation after exposure of virgin males to the Hbb-bt/Hba-a1 complexes (Figure 5G). Interestingly, neurons expressing Vmn2r122/123 were also activated by Hbb-bt/Hbb-a1 (Figure 5G). Finally, Hbb-bt was also identified by MS in pup salivary gland extract fractions with Vmn2r88 ligand activity (Supplemental figure 5C,D,E), demonstrating that Hb represents the Vmn2r88 ligand activity in both adults and pups.

The Hb beta gene locus in mice exists in two major haplotypes, Hbbs and Hbbd. The Hbbs haplotype of the C57BL/6J strain is defined by the Hbb-bt gene while the Hbbd haplotype of the 129S1/SvImJ strain is represented by the Hbb-b1 and Hbb-b2 genes (Hempe et al., 2007). Because Hbb-bt and Hbb-b1 sequences differ in only three amino acids, we wondered if Vmn2r88 can discriminate between these two ligands. We expressed Hbb-b1 and Hbb-b2, each complexed with Hba-a1 in E. coli, and exposed virgin male mice to these purified protein complexes at the same concentration as in our initial test with Hbb-bt/Hba-a1 (0.4 mg at 4 mg/ml). Notably, this concentration is over an order of magnitude below the reported concentration of Hb in the blood of adult mice (~140 mg/ml) (Raabe et al., 2011) and therefore likely falls within a physiological range as a VNO stimulus. Interestingly, in contrast to the robust activation by Hbb-bt/Hba-a1 (Figure 5G), we observed a much weaker activation of Vmn2r88-expressing neurons by Hbb-b1/Hba-a1 and no apparent activation by Hbb-b2/Hba-a1 (Figure 5H): Egr1 signals in Vmn2r88-expressing neurons in VNOs of animals exposed to Hbb-b2/Hba-a1 did not significantly exceed the background (1.77 ± 0.13 [g2026] above background, N = 3~4 per stimulus), and this activity was significantly weaker than the activities of Hbb-bt/Hba-a1 and Hbb-b1/Hba-a1 as Vmn2r88 ligands (p < 0.001; Figure 5I). These results suggest that Hbs emitted by several mouse strains may act as VNO cues, and that the strength of signaling through Vmn2r88-expressing neurons is strain-dependent. In addition, the specificity of the Vmn2r88 stimulating activity seems to reside in the beta subunit of the Hb complex.

The finding that Vmn2r88 is activated by Hb is highly unexpected, since Hb, although reported to be expressed in tissues other than mature and immature red blood cells (Gomes et al., 2010), is not known to be actively secreted outside the body in the absence of bleeding. Moreover, because attacks by males will naturally release Hb through pup wounding, activation of Vmn2r88 may result from, rather than be the cause of, male aggression. We have shown that Vmn2r88 is also robustly activated in virgin females exposed to pups, suggesting that Hb is naturally presented by pups even in the absence of male attacks (Supplemental figure 2B). These considerations prompted us to further examine the origin of this chemosignal.

Blood from both male and female adult C57BL/6J mice strongly activated Vmn2r88 expressing neurons (Figure 5C). In addition, several lines of evidence suggest that Hb is constitutively deposited in mouse bedding in absence of overt bleeding. First, the Vmn2r88 ligand is enriched in the mixed bedding of adult male mice from five strains (Isogai et al., 2011). Second, we found that bedding from group-housed adult C57BL/6J males, but not from adult virgin C57BL/6J females, activates Vmn2r88 neurons (Figure 5J). Third, strong Vmn2r88 ligand activity was found in bedding of co-housed pups and mothers, and in pups presented alone, while the ligand activity is not detectable in the bedding of C57BL/6J mothers after they were transferred to new cages with fresh bedding and without pups for two days (Figure 5J).

Strikingly, we consistently identified Hbs in the beddings of mothers cohoused with P1 pups by western blot, while Hbs were not detected in the bedding of similarly single housed non-pregnant females and males (Figure 5L). The observation that Vmn2r88-stimulating activity is found in the bedding of mother with pups could indicate the presence of remnant blood following parturition. This hypothesis prompted us to test the stability of Hb over days. Indeed, we found that hemoglobin’s ability to activate Vmn2r88 remains stable for at least three weeks (Figure 5M, N), consistent with the notion of Hb signals able to persist from parturition until weaning.

Finally, in order to determine whether Hbs found in the bedding of co-housed pups and mothers originate from pups, mothers, or both, we tested bedding from cross-fostered pups. Since Hbb-b1, expressed by the 129S1/SvImJ strain, is less active in stimulating Vmn2r88 neurons than Hbb-bt, expressed by C57BL/6J mice, transfer of Hbs from C57BL/6J pups should be detectable in bedding of 129S1/SvImJ mother’s cages. However, cross-fostering C57BL/6J pups with 129S1/SvImJ mothers did not increase Vmn2r88 stimulating activity of bedding, while bedding of cross-fostered 129S1/SvImJ pups with C57BL/6J mothers did (Figure 5K). This indicates that mothers, might be a more significant source of Hbs than pups, likely deposited during parturition.

Hbs thus emerge as a new class of chemosignals which, since highly correlated with the birth and continuing presence of pups until weaning, can be exploited by males. Hbs, together with Smgc, are the first compounds isolated based on their participation in pup signaling to males. However, our attempts to swab recombinant Hbs and Smgc alone and together onto pup dummies did not result in any apparent increase in male aggression. This result indicated that the sole or combined activation of Vmn2r65, Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r122/123-expressing neurons from the seven receptor populations detecting pup cues is insufficient to fully represent the complex blend of pup signals leading to infanticide. Future studies may take advantage of the experimental system developed here to identify ligands corresponding to the other pup-activated VRs, i.e., V1rc1, V1rc30 and V1ri9.

Contribution of two V2Rs in pup-directed aggression

To further assess the significance of Hbs and Smgc as pheromonal compounds, we genetically tested the involvement of the corresponding receptors in pup-directed aggression and generated loss-of-function alleles for the two major receptors activated by Smgc and Hbs, Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 (Methods). While not fully abrogating pup detection, combined mutations in these two VR genes, out of the seven VRs responding to infant cues, should reduce the number of VNO neurons activated by pup cues by more than 50% (Figure 2H), and may thus lead to observable effects on pup-mediated behavior compared to wild type animals.

Vmn2r65−/− and Vmn2r88−/− alleles lacking the seven trans-membrane domain were generated by homologous recombination (Figure 6A,B), resulting in complete absence of Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 transcripts in mutant VNOs (Figure 6C,D). We tested the resulting virgin male mutants for pup-directed behaviors.

Figure 6: Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r65 contribution to pup-directed aggression by virgin adult males.

Figure 6:

(A, B) Schematic of VR knockout constructs. The targeted (A) Vmn2r65 and (B) Vmn2r88 loci lack the transmembrane domain of the corresponding receptor.

(C, D) RNA FISH using probes specific to Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 (Isogai et al., 2011) confirms the loss of Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 expression in the VNO of Vmn2r65−/− and Vmn2r88−/− mice, respectively.

(E) Pup directed behaviors are altered in receptor knockout virgin males. Each circle represents a single animal, and the assignment of each animal in three behavioral outcomes is unique. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 by Fisher’s Exact Test.

(F) Number of non-parental animals is significantly different between mice in which specific receptor inputs are deleted. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 by Fisher’s Exact Test.

(G) Detailed quantitative analysis of pup-directed behaviors. The full analysis from all genotypes is presented in Supplemental figure 6A.

All scale bars represent 100 μm.

The behavior of Vmn2r65−/− males differed significantly from littermate controls (p < 0.05, Fisher’s Exact Test, N = 36 for wild type males; N = 35 for Vmn2r65−/− males; Figure 6E), with a decrease in pup-directed aggression in Vmn2r65−/− compared to Vmn2r65+/+ and Vmn2r65+/− virgin males. In addition, an increased number of Vmn2r65−/− animals compared to littermate controls displayed parenting, a response rarely observed in wild type virgin males (2.8 % of wild type males, N = 36 versus 28.6 % of Vmn2r65−/− males, N = 35; Figure 6E).

By contrast, we found no significant difference in pup-mediated attack versus ignoring or retrieving pups in Vmn2r88−/− compared to Vmn2r88+/+ and Vmn2r88+/− virgin males (p = 0.11, Fisher’s Exact Test; N = 31 for wild type males; N = 29 for Vmn2r88−/− male, p = 0.48, Fisher’s Exact Test; N = 29 for Vmn2r88+/− males; N = 29 for Vmn2r88−/− males; Figure 6E). Because Hbs were found to activate both Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r122/123, (Figure 5G) the deletion of the sole Vmn2r88 gene may thus be insufficient to abrogate detection of these cues and impair infanticide.

Next, we analyzed the pup-directed behaviors of double mutant virgin males (Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/−), and found that they dramatically differed from those of wild type males (p < 0.01, Fisher’s Exact Test; Figure 6E). Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− virgin males displayed a robust reduction in pup-directed aggression compared to wild type controls (53.4 % of wild type males combined for Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 groups, N = 67, versus 16.7 % of Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− males, N = 30; Figure 6E), while acquiring a significant but modest increase in parental behavior (3.0 % of wild type males combined for Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 groups, N = 67, versus 23.3 % of double knockout males, N = 30). Further, 60.0 % of the double mutants (N = 30) failed to exhibit either aggressive or parental behaviors after 30 minutes, even though they extensively chemo-investigated the pups (Figure 6E). Moreover, the difference in the behaviors towards pups between Vmn2r88−/− and Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− mice was statistically significant (p = 0.003, Fisher’s Exact Test), and non-parental behaviors of Vmn2r65−/− and Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− virgin males were also significantly different between the two groups (p = 0.041, Fisher’s Exact Test; Figure 6F), with fewer aggressive animals in the Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− cohort than Vmn2r65−/− alone. These results point to a contribution of the Vmn2r88 deletion in pup-directed aggression when combined with the Vmn2r65−/− mutation, and suggest an additivity of receptor functions in the control of pup-directed behavior.

Importantly, we noted that the phenotype of Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− virgin males was not equivalent to that of animals completely deprived of VNO function, such as Trpc2−/− (Wu et al., 2014) and VNO-ablated males (Tachikawa et al., 2013), which, in addition to the lack of pup directed attack, display robust parental behaviors. Because infant signals are still detected in Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− males through the remaining VRs detecting pups, full display of parenting behavior in these mutants may be hindered by residual VNO signaling.

Detailed analysis of pup-directed behavior in mutants compared to controls (Figure 6G; Supplemental figure 6A) showed that, while the number of animals attacking versus ignoring and parenting was significantly different between genotypes (Figure 6E), there was no quantitative difference for several other measures such as attack latency, duration and latency of parental behaviors (pup retrieval, nest building, crouching over pups, and chemo-investigation of animals showing neither aggression nor retrieval) (Figure 6G; Supplemental figure 6A). This suggests that pheromone inputs may affect the initial behavioral choice rather than the parameters of behavior execution.

Next, we tested Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− double mutants for deficits in other social behaviors. VNO neurons expressing Vmn2r65 are activated by female bedding (Isogai et al., 2011), prompting us to test Vmn2r65−/− males for deficits in mating behavior. However, we found no apparent deficit in a variety of parameters associated with mating, such as chemo-investigation, mounting and intromission (Supplemental figure 6B), suggesting that Vmn2r65 is not essential for male-female interactions. Further, we assayed inter-male aggression in double knockout males using a resident intruder test (Supplemental figure 6C). The percentage of Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− males attacking intruders in 30 min sessions showed a slight but not statistically significant decrease from that of control animals (p = 0.20, Fisher’s Exact Test; 70.0 % for double knockouts, 92.3 % for wild type controls, N = 13 wild type, N = 20 Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/−). In addition, attack duration was indistinguishable from wild type control males (p = 0.36, Student’s t-Test, N = 13 wild type, N = 20 Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/−). Further, we found minor effects in the latency of aggression and duration of chemoinvestigative interactions in these animals, including sniffing and grooming. These results suggest that, in contrast to their strong impairment in pup-mediated aggression, Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− males only display minor defects in aggression towards adult males.

Taken together, virgin males lacking the ability to sense two specific sets of VNO ligands associated with pups exhibit impaired pup-directed aggression, with a seemingly additive effect of the two receptor mutations. These results suggest an unexpected but efficient solution to the challenge of pup-recognition by virgin males: VNO cues emitted by pups are ambiguous and males must thus rely on the detection of multiple chemosensory cues, together with additional sources of information associated with the presence of pups. Our study demonstrates and identifies the integration of multiple cues – VNO receptor inputs and information about shape – in generating the appropriate response of virgin males towards infants.

Discussion

In this study, we examined the sensory triggers of infanticide by adult males, a dramatic yet poorly understood behavior observed throughout the animal kingdom. Aggression toward infants is displayed by sexually mature intruder males in many species of mammals, and has been extensively studied by ethologists and evolutionary biologists in the context of sexual selection (Hausfater and Hrdy, 1984). This physiological and evolutionary framework, initially developed from the observation of langurs (Sugiyama, 1965; Hrdy, 1974), and further expanded to a variety of primates, lions, rodents, and about half of mammalian species (Lukas and Huchard, 2014), postulates that males seek to increase their reproductive success by killing the offspring of rival males. In turn, the newly infant-deprived mother re-enters the estrus cycle (Hausfater and Hrdy, 1984; McCarthy and Vom Saal, 1986), enabling the intruder male to sire its own offspring.

In mice and rats, existing literature suggests that different types of infant signals initiate either parental care or infanticide (Beach and Jaynes, 1956; Stern, 1990; Tachikawa et al., 2013; Fraser and Shah, 2014; Wu et al., 2014; Marlin et al., 2015). In these species infant olfactory, auditory, and tactile cues can elicit maternal responses (Beach and Jaynes, 1956) (Stern, 1990) (Marlin et al., 2015). Interestingly, deprivation of a single sensory modality has little effect on maternal responses, while deprivation of two or more senses has more severe effects, suggesting that pup recognition by mothers involves the identification of multiple and partially redundant infant signals (Beach and Jaynes, 1956). By contrast, female and male mice in which VNO function is genetically or surgically impaired display largely normal parental behaviors, suggesting that VNO inputs are not required, although they may still participate in infant recognition (Kimchi et al., 2007; Fraser and Shah, 2014).

In mice, the understanding of pup recognition by infanticidal males, although scant, points to a set of cues distinct from those required to trigger parenting. Male mice lacking VNO activity display dramatically reduced aggression towards pups and instead frequently adopt parental behaviors (Mennella and Moltz, 1988; Tachikawa et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2014). This indicates that VNO-mediated infant cues and their cognate receptors, although not required for parental responses, are essential for pup-directed male aggression.

Here, we have used an immediate early gene-based high-throughput assay (Isogai et al., 2011), as well as an independent method based on the phosphorylation of the ribosomal protein S6 in activated neurons (Knight et al., 2012), to uncover the repertoire of seven VRs that together encompass 92.2% of the response to pup cues in aggressive virgin males. Moreover, we demonstrated genetically that two of these receptors, representing over 50% of the VNO response to pup signals, are involved in an additive manner in the sensory detection leading to pup-mediated aggression. These data uncovered an intriguing paradox: although VNO activation by pup cues is required to trigger typical pup-directed aggression by virgin males, the corresponding receptors are promiscuous, additionally recognizing cues from adult males or females, which convey very different types of social information associated with distinct behavioral responses.

These findings provide novel insights into the neurobiology of infant recognition (Figure 7; Supplemental figure 7). First, it appears that neonates do not emit any infant-specific VNO ligands. This is perhaps unsurprising since unambiguous pup-specific identification by the VNO would be predicted to lead to increased attack by virgin males and thus likely be selected against. Instead, VNO cues associated with pups seem to result from mother-infant cohabitation. We found that Smgc and hemoglobins are abundant in nests of mothers nurturing infants. These chemosignals could therefore be directly transferred to pups.

Figure 7: Pup recognition by virgin males involves multi-sensory inputs.

Figure 7:

A model of vomeronasal signals associated with pups. Chemical signals associated with the presence of infants in fact originates from both pups and mothers.

Despite the absence of non-ambiguous, pup-specific VNO ligands, VNO activation is required for the expression of infanticide (Tachikawa et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2014). Two models may explain the specificity of behavioral responses by aggressive males. It is possible that the simultaneous activation of receptors identifying adult males and females conveys an unusual signal that triggers attack. Alternatively, or in addition to, aggressive males may detect additional non-VNO cues that lead to the unequivocal recognition of infants and attack.

Indeed, a reductionist behavioral paradigm based on pup-shaped silicone dummies, showed that the combined detection of chemical and shape-mediated signals triggers robust expression of pup-directed aggression. By contrast, several other sensory modalities, such as auditory, motion and temperature cues appear dispensable for pup-directed aggression, although these signals may help in further strengthening the aggressive response of males to pups, as suggested by the higher intensity of attacks triggered by live compared to dead infants and scented dummies.

Works by classical ethologists on instinctive behaviors in rats, particularly male mating and female parenting (Beach, 1942b; Beach and Jaynes, 1956) have suggested that, in contrast with the unimodal sign stimuli described by Tinbergen in insects, fish and birds (Tinbergen, 1969), information originating from multiple cues and sensory modalities is required to drive innate behavior in mammals. Similarly, we have uncovered the sensory integration of multiple pheromonal cues, none of which unique to pups, together with pup-mediated somatosensory inputs, as underlying instinctive male infanticide. A model of simple stimuli driving specific behaviors through dedicated labeled-line neuronal pathways may therefore not easily apply to mammals.

In mice, a handful of monomolecular chemical cues have been identified that play important roles in VNO pheromonal responses (Chamero et al., 2007; Haga et al., 2010; Ferrero et al., 2013; Fu et al., 2015). However, with the exception of major urinary proteins promoting territorial marking by males (Kaur et al., 2014), live animals are in many cases necessary to trigger pheromonal actions, thus leaving intact the contribution of multiple sensory cues in triggering behavior output. The reductionist approach developed here should allow for the identification of a comprehensive set of sensory information required to elicit distinct social behaviors.

The discovery of hemoglobins (Hbs) as a new family of VNO receptor ligands is both highly unexpected and intriguing. Hb beta t chain (Hbb-bt) is ubiquitously expressed in C57BL/6J mice at all stages of development and in both sexes (Weaver et al., 1981). Surprisingly, the activity of Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r122/123 mediated by Hbb-bt is found in C57BL/6J pups and in the bedding of adult C57BL/6J males and that of mothers and pups, but not of virgin females. The origin of Hbs in mouse bedding may be two-fold. It is possible that the Hbs deposited in bedding that stimulate Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r122/123 are metabolized byproducts from mothers, pups, or both. Indeed, it has been reported that secreted peptides derived from Hbs play physiological roles (Gomes et al., 2010). In addition, bleeding occurs in a number of related social contexts, particularly parturition. Indeed, blood spots in bedding of mothers who recently delivered pups can often be observed, likely originating from extraembryonic tissues. Importantly, these events take place independently from bleeding from pups resulting from male aggression. Therefore, somewhat unexpectedly, Hbs are ubiquitous in certain social environments and may be exploited as pheromones. Finally, pup bleeding caused by male attacks may further elevate the responses of Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r122/123 expressing neurons, and thus escalate aggressive responses of males to wounded pups. The high sensitivity of the Vmn2r88 and Vmn2r122/123 receptors in turn appears sufficient for the detection of Hbs at concentrations well below its level in blood.

In conclusion, our study opens new avenues to study the circuit basis underlying the recognition of pups leading to infanticide by males, it uncovers precise physical and chemical features underlying this behavioral response, highlights the combination of multiple sensory modalities in eliciting a stereotyped but physiologically flexible instinctive behavior, and uncovers unusual sensory signaling from infants in a species in which adults can be both nurturing or aggressive.

STAR METHODS

KEY RESOURCES TABLE

CONTACT FOR REAGENT AND RESOURCE SHARING

Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to, and will be fulfilled by, Catherine Dulac (dulac@fas.harvard.edu).

EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS

Mice

Animals were maintained on 12 hr:12hr light/dark cycle with food and water provided ad libitum. Care of animals and experiments were performed in accordance with NIH guidelines and approved by the Harvard University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC). Only healthy animals were used for this study. Behavior experiments were performed in the dark phase of the light/dark cycle. In addition, all animals were initially group-housed in single sex cages post weaning and subsequently single housed prior to behavior experiments. For experiments described in Figure 1, male TrpC2+/− mice were used. These animals were single housed starting at 4 months old. For stimulus exposure experiments described in Figures 2,3,4, and 5, CD-1 males and females older than 4 weeks were used. For experiments described in Figure 6, all behavioral tests with receptor knockout mice, knockouts and littermate controls, including wild types and heterozygotes were performed with 3 to 6 month old animals. Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 single knockout males as well as littermate wild type and heterozygous control males were generated by heterozygous crosses. Vmn2r65−/−; Vmn2r88−/− mice were generated using homozygous as well as heterozygous crosses.

METHOD DETAILS

RNA ANALYSIS

RNA in situ hybridization

Chromogenic in situ hybridization developed with NBT/BCIP was performed as previously described (Isogai et al., 2011). Fluorescence RNA in situ hybridization was performed as previously detailed (Isogai et al., 2011), except for use of either FITC or Dinitrophenyl (DNP) labeled Egr1 probe and DIG labeled vomeronasal receptor probes. Sequences of probe targets are found in the Supplemental Table 1. Reagents used for RNA in situ hybridization are also listed in the Key Resources Table.

KEY RESOURCES TABLE

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER
Antibodies
Phospho-S6 Ribosomal Protein (Ser240/244) Antibody Cell Signaling Technology 2215BF
Anti-digoxigenin-POD Roche 11207733910
Anti-fluorescein-POD Roche 11426346910
Anti-DNP-HRP Perkin Elmer FP1129
Anti-rabbit IgG HRP-linked antibody Cell Signaling Technology 7074P2
Rabbit anti-HBB (, PA5–48233) Invitrogen PA5–48233
Donkey anti-rabbit Alexa555 conjugate Invitrogen A-31572
Streptavidin-Alexa488 conjugate Invitrogen or Jackson ImmunoResearch S11223 or 016–540-084
Bacterial and Virus Strains
E. coli Rosetta BL2 (DE3) Novagen 70954
Biological Samples
Vmn2r65 targeted null mice This paper
Vmn2r88 targeted null mice This paper
Vmn2r65/Vmn2r88 targeted null mice This paper
Chemicals, Peptides, and Recombinant Proteins
FLAG (DYKDDDDK) peptide Genscript RP10586
T7 RNA polymerase New England Biolabs or Promega M0251S/L or P2075
Sp6 RNA polymerase New England Biolabs or Promega M0207S/L or P1085
T3 RNA polymerase Promega P2083
NBT/BCIP Promega S3771
Dig RNA labeling mix Roche 11277073910
Fluorescein RNA labeling mix Roche 11685619910
DNP-11-UTP Perkin Elmer NEL555001EA
Magnetic protein A dynabeads Invitrogen 10001D
Critical Commercial Assays
TSA plus biotin kit Perkin Elmer NEL749A001KT
TSA plus Cy3 kit Perkin Elmer NEL744001KT
SMARTer Ultra Low Input RNA for Illumina Sequencing - HV Clontech 634826
Low Input Library Prep Kit Clontech 634947
Deposited Data
M. musculus reference genome mm10 Ensembl ftp://ftp.ensembl.org/pub/release-73/fasta/mus_musculus/dna/
RNA-seq raw and analyzed data This study GEO: GSE122138
Experimental Models: Cell Lines
Experimental Models: Organisms/Strains
Mus musculus, C57BL6/J The Jackson Laboratory Stock No:000664
Mus musculus, 129S1/SvImJ The Jackson Laboratory Stock No:002448
Mus musculus, CD-1 Charles River N/A
Mus musculus, TrpC2+/- Stowers et al. 2002 Trpc2tm1Dlc
Oligonucleotides
Vmn2r65 wild type genotyping primer, forward CTTCATAGCGTTTTTGCCTTC This study N/A
Vmn2r65 wild type genotyping primer, reverse TGAGAAGTCGGTTATTCGCC This study N/A
Vmn2r65 mutant genotyping primer, forward TAACGGTCCTAAGGTAGCGA This study N/A
Vmn2r65 mutant genotyping primer, reverse TCACCCTGAATGCAGGTGTA This study N/A
Vmn2r88 wild type genotyping primer, forward GTTTTGTTTGATCAGAAACATAGGTTAATAC This study N/A
Vmn2r88 wild type genotyping primer, reverse AGTTCTGTCCTGTTTGTAAGTTCAAATTTGT This study N/A
Vmn2r88 mutant genotyping primer, forward TAACGGTCCTAAGGTAGCGA This study N/A
Vmn2r88 mutant genotyping primer, reverse TTTTGGTTGAGGACTGGAGT This study N/A
Recombinant DNA
pET28a-hbb-bt-SD-hba-a1-his This study N/A
pET28a-his-smgc-flag This study N/A
Software and Algorithms
RNA STAR 2.3.1 Dobin et al. 2013 https://github.com/alexdobin/STAR
DESeq2 Love et al. 2014 https://bioconductor.org/packages/release/bioc/html/DESeq2.html
Rstudio https://www.rstudio.com/ RRID:SCR_000432
R http://www.r-project.org/ RRID: SCR_001905
Fiji http://fiji.sc RRID: SCR_002285
Samtools http://samtools.sourceforge.net/ RRID:SCR_002105
Prism 5 Graphpad N/A
Observer XT 14 Noldus N/A
Other
Body Double Standard Set Smooth-On N/A

Phospho-S6 pulldown

The immunoprecipitation of phosphorylated polysomes from vomeronasal organs was performed as described in Knight et al. 2012. Briefly, VNOs were pooled from as many as five CD-1 adult males (Charles River) exhibiting pup directed aggression, and tissue homogenates were prepared in the presence of RNase inhibitors (RNasin, Promega; SUPERase•In, Life Technologies) and Protease and Phosphatase Inhibitors (Halt, Pierce; Calyculin A, Santa Cruz Biotechnology). The extract cleared by detergent (DHPC and NP-40) treatment and high-speed centrifugation was mixed with Protein A Dynabeads (Invitrogen), pre-incubated with anti-pS6 (240/244) antibody (Cell Signaling Technology) and incubated for 10 min at 4 °C. After high salt washes, the bound polysomes were eluted and purified using Absolutely RNA Nanoprep Kit (Agilent). The blocking peptide for Phospho-S235/236 was not included, since antibodies specific to Phospho-S235/236 of the ribosomal protein S6 did not label any neurons in the VNO unless the cells were stimulated. As controls for this assay, pulldowns were performed with VNOs from animals presented with fresh bedding. The experiments were done in triplicates.

RNA sequencing

SMARTer Ultra Low Input RNA for Illumina Sequencing - HV and Low Input Library Prep Kit (Clontech) were used to prepare RNA sequencing libraries. The cDNA libraries were sequenced by Illumina HiSeq 2500 using 50 bp paired end, rapid flow cells, which produced 15~20 million reads per sample.

PURIFICATION OF VOMERONASAL LIGANDS

Extract preparation

Salivary glands (containing both submaxillary and sublingual glands but not preputial gland) were dissected from adult (5 weeks to 1 year old) mice of strains such as C57BL/6J. The glands from males and females were collected separately and served for extract preparation. Pup glands were dissected from C57BL/6J pups (P1~P6), which consisted of both males and females. 129S1/SvImJ salivary glands were also dissected and processed identically. Approximately 1 gram of tissue was homogenized in 2 ml of 1× PBS supplemented with cOmplete EDTA-free Protease Inhibitor Cocktail tablet (Roche), 0.25 μM PMSF, 10 μg/ml AEBSF and 0.5 mM DTT in 4 °C. After centrifugation in 15,000 rpm for 30 min, the supernatant was transferred to a beaker in which a bulk of the proteins was precipitated with ammonium sulfate (below).

Ammonium sulfate precipitation

Ammonium sulfate precipitation was performed using standard procedures. Briefly, 0.23 grams of ammonium sulfate were added to achieve 40 % saturation at 4 °C. After the crystals were completely dissolved upon a 1 hr mixing on ice, the precipitate was pelleted by centrifugation at 4 °C. Since the super natant retained the activity, this fraction was filtered using 0.45 μm PES syringe filter (Millipore) and the ammonium sulfate was removed by either dialysis to 1× PBS or by buffer exchange using a spin column (Amicon Ultra-0.5 mL Centrifugal Filters, 10 kDa cutoff, Millipore).

Gel filtration chromatography

Sephacryl S100HR (GE Healthcare Life Sciences) was connected to PerSeptive Biosystems BioCAD Vision Workstation (Applied Biosystems) and 1× PBS was used as the column buffer. Approximately 1~2 ml of supernatant after the ammonium sulfate step was concentrated by Amicon Ultra-0.5 mL Centrifugal Filters and injected into the column. The protein compositions of every third fractions of this column were analyzed on SDS-PAGE gel. Subsequently select fractions were assayed for ligand activity by the exposure of 10 μl fractions to mice (details below).

Analytical gel filtration chromatography shown in the Supplemental figure 5 was carried out in Infinity II 1260 system (Agilent) with Bio Sec-3 (300A, 78×300 mm, Agilent) column with 1× PBS (pH 7.4) at room temperature as a mobile phase (1 ml/min). 5 μl of hemoglobins were injected into the column with an autosampler, and hemoglobins were monitored using 405 and 561 nm.

Ni Sepharose binding of Vmn2r88 ligand activity

The peak activity fractions from the gel filtration step were incubated with Ni Sepharose 6 Fast Flow resin (GE Healthcare) for 3 hrs at 4 °C with gentle mixing. After >10 column volume wash with 1× PBS/0.1 % NP-40, bound proteins were eluted using a step elution 20 mM, 50 mM, and 100 mM imidazole in 1× PBS, 4 column volume each. Most of the Vmn2r88 stimulating activity eluted in 50 mM imidazole fractions, and these fractions were pooled and buffer exchanged for further purification by Poros HQ column.

Ion exchange chromatography

For DEAE chromatography, 1 ml HiTrap DEAE Fast Flow (GE Healthcare) and 50 mM Tris pH 7.5 were used as the column binding buffer. The supernatant from the ammonium sulfate precipitation step was buffer exchanged to 50 mM Tris pH 7.5 and concentrated using Amicon Ultra-0.5 mL Centrifugal Filters. The bound proteins were eluted by the salt gradient to 2 M NaCl in the same buffer, and 200 μl fractions were collected.

For strong anion exchange chromatography to fractionate the Vmn2r65 ligand activity, we used Poros HQ/M column (1.7 ml, 20 μm, ThermoFisher Scientific) and 50 mM Tris pH 7.5 as a binding buffer at room temperature. For the purification of Vmn2r88 ligand activity, we used 50 mM Tris pH 8.8 at 4 °C for bin ding. Bound proteins were eluted using the gradient to 2 M NaCl in binding buffers. 200 μl fractions were collected and served for the analysis by SDS-PAGE and activity assays.

Activity testing

Throughout the chromatographic steps, 10 μl of fractions were presented to adult CD-1 male mice. Their VNOs were analyzed for the Egr1 induction in Vmn2r65, Vmn2r88, or Vmn2r122/123 expressing cells.

Mass spectrometry

The mass spectrometry was conducted at the Harvard University FAS Mass Spectrometry Facility. The following conditions were used to identify proteins in purified chromatographic fractions. The samples were first buffer exchanged to 0.1 mM tetraethylammonium bicarbonate. The fractions were then treated with TCEP and iodoacetamide, followed by digestion with trypsin in solution overnight at 37 °C. Digested samples were submitted for LC-MS/MS performed by LTQ Orbitrap Elite (ThermoFisher Scientific) equipped with Waters NanoAcquity HPLC pump. The mass spectrometry survey scan was performed in the Orbitrap in the range of 395–1,800 m/z at a resolution of 6 × 104, followed by the selection of the twenty most intense ions (TOP20) for CID-MS2 fragmentation in the Ion trap using a precursor isolation width window of 2 m/z, AGC setting of 10,000, and a maximum ion accumulation of 200 ms. Singly charged ion species were not subjected to CID fragmentation. Normalized collision energy was set to 35 V and an activation time of 10 ms, AGC was set to 50,000, the maximum ion time was 200 ms. Ions in a 10 ppm m/z window around ions selected for MS2 were excluded from further selection for fragmentation for 60 s.

Raw data were submitted for analysis in Proteome Discoverer 2.1.0.81 (ThermoFisher Scientific) software. Assignment of MS/MS spectra was performed using the Sequest HT algorithm by searching the data against a protein sequence database including all entries from the Mouse Uniprot Database and other known contaminants such as human keratins and common lab contaminants. Sequest HT searches were performed using a 20 ppm precursor ion tolerance and requiring each peptides N/C termini to adhere with Trypsin protease specificity while allowing up to two missed cleavages. Cysteine carbamidomethyl (+57.021) was set as static modifications while methionine oxidation (+15.99492 Da) was set as variable modification. MS2 spectra assignment false discovery rate (FDR) of 1% on protein level was achieved by applying the target-decoy database search. Filtering was performed using a Percolator (64bit version, reference 6). For quantification, a 0.02 m/z window centered on the theoretical m/z value of each the six reporter ions and the intensity of the signal closest to the theoretical m/z value was recorded. Reporter ion intensities were exported as excel tables. All fold changes were analyzed after normalization between samples based on total unique peptides ion signal.

Expression and purification of recombinant Smgc

A codon optimized cDNA corresponding to the isoform 3 of murine Smgc (Uniprot ID: Q6JHY2–2) lacking the signal peptide sequence (aa1–20) was synthesized (Genscript) and inserted as N-terminal histidine and C-terminal flag tags into pET28a vector (Novagen). The expression of Smgc was carried out in Rosetta (DE3) cells (Novagen) and was induced with 100 μM IPTG overnight at 20 °C. The pellet was resuspended in 1× PBS containing 0.25 μM PMSF and lysed using lysozyme and repeated sonication. The lysate was cleared by centrifugation, adjusted to 25 mM imidazole, and incubated with Ni Sepharose 6 Fast Flow resin (GE Healthcare Life Sciences) for at least 3 hours at 4 °C. After >20× column volume wash with 1× PBS added with 30 mM imidazole and 300 mM NaCl, bound proteins were eluted with 250 mM imidazole in 1× PBS at pH8. The peak fractions were subsequently incubated with Flag M2 magnetic beads (Sigma) for 3 hrs to overnight at 4 °C. Subsequent ten wash es with 1× PBS/0.1 % NP-40 (Calbiochem), bound proteins were eluted with one bead volume of 0.4 mM of FLAG peptide (Genscript) in 1× PBS three times by room temperature incubation for 30 min with gentle mixing. The purity and the molecular weight of the protein were analyzed by SDS-PAGE.

Expression and purification of recombinant hemoglobins

Codon optimized cDNAs comprising of C terminal histidine tagged Hba-a1 (Uniprot ID: P01942), a linker containing Shine-Dalgarno sequence (TGActgcagctacatggagattaactcaatctAGAGGGtattaataatgtatcgcttaaataAGGAGGAataacat), and either mouse Hbb-bt (Uniprot ID: A8DUK4), Hbb-b1 (Uniprot ID: P02088), or Hbb-b2 (Uniprot ID: P02089) were synthesized (IDT) and inserted to pET28a. The protein expression was induced in Rosetta (DE3) cells (Novagen) by 0.25 mM IPTG for 3 hours at 30 °C. Notably, the recombinant proteins had a r eddish color, suggesting that recombinant hemoglobins are bound by endogenous heme-like molecules. We found the yield and activity of the recombinant Hba-a1/Hbb-bt comparable whether or not hemin was added to the culture. The lysate from the induced culture was purified similarly using the condition for Smgc using Ni Sepharose 6 Fast Flow and the purified proteins were analyzed by SDS-PAGE. The concentration of the purified proteins was adjusted to 4 mg/ml in 1× PBS.

Western blot

Western blot was carried out using bedding extracts of 129S1/SvImJ. One male and female were paired for breeding, and just prior to the parturition, the male was separated from the cage and we refreshed nesting material (a cotton pad). On the day pups are born, we sampled the bedding exclusively the nest. Especially, to enrich the protein concentration of the bedding extract, we limited the sampling to visibly stained materials (~40 μl in volume). We then used 40 μl of 1× PBS to extract proteins from these bedding by 15 min incubation at room temperature. Pregnant female bedding was collected one to two days prior to parturition. Therefore, the nesting material was in contact with the females for 1~2 days. Control bedding samples from non-pregnant females and males (all single housed) was sampled similarly after one to two days of exposure to the nesting material. 15 % SDS-PAGE. The proteins were subsequently transferred to nitrocellulose membranes (Amersham Protran Premium Western Blotting Membranes, GE Healthcare). Western blots were performed using 1:3000 rabbit anti-HBB (Invitrogen, PA5–48233) in 5 % powdered milk/1xPBST as the primary antibody and 1:10000 anti-rabbit IgG HRP-linked antibody (Cell Signaling Technologies, 7074P2) in 5 % powdered milk/1xPBST as the secondary antibody. The chemiluminescence was developed using SuperSignal West Pico PLUS Chemiluminescent Substrate (Thermo Fisher) and was captured using Azure Imager c400 (Azure Biosystems).

BEHAVIOR TESTS

Odor exposure assays

Vomeronasal stimulation via odor and pup exposure was conducted using methods already described (Isogai et al., 2011). The odor exposure experiments were performed in single housed cages. For bedding or pup exposure, we introduced bedding materials or pups into the cage, which induced frequent direct sniffing. For the presentation of liquids, 10 μl of the solutions were directly spotted to the nostril. VNOs were dissected after 40 min post stimulus presentation and were embedded to OCT and frozen in dry ice. For the experiments conducted in Figure 2D, 30 min was chosen for the exposure duration. These samples were subsequently served for RNA in situ hybridization analysis. Experiments in Figure 2AC used TrpC2+/− mice whereas all other experiments used sexually naive CD-1 male and female mice (Charles River, adults older than at least 4 weeks). The induction of Egr1 was used as a molecular marker for neural activity in the VNO.

Stimulus sampling for odor exposure assays

Bedding:

Bedding consists of ground corncob and shredded cotton pads, which mice use to build their nest. For adult mouse bedding, soiled bedding was collected from cages that socially housed 4 to 5 adult mice of specific strains for 1 week. Then the bedding of 50 ml in volume, making sure that both urinary, fecal and other nest-associated excretions were equally represented, was used for each experiment. To sample chemosignals associated with mothers co-housed with pups, bedding specifically around their nests was used. The cage typically housed two C57BL/6J lactating mothers, which gave birth to C57BL/6J in a new cage with fresh bedding without mated males, and P1~P4 pups. For mother’s bedding, C57BL/6J mothers were single housed for 2 days and the nesting materials with which mothers had extensive contact as well as soiled bedding for exposure experiments were selectively sampled and used for the exposure.

Pups:

C57BL/6J pups (P1 to P5) were exclusively used for the experiments. Pregnant C57BL/6J females previously mated with C57BL/6J males were transferred to a new cage with fresh bedding. Notably, these cages did not come in contact with adult males, to eliminate the contribution of adult male-derived chemosignals on pups. All experiments used 1 male and 1 female pups.

Cross-fostered bedding:

first, 129S1/SvImJ females were mated with 129S1/SvImJ males and C57BL/6J females were mated with C57BL/6J males. Pregnant females were single housed prior to parturition. Pups in these cages were then introduced to either 129S1/SvImJ or C57BL/6J mother’s cages for cross-fostering. Each cage received 5 to 8 pups and the pups were cross-fostered for 4 to 5 days. The bedding from the nest area was collected and exposed to CD-1 males. 3 independent bedding samples were tested per group.

Gland extracts:

Gland extracts were made by homogenization of select glands (Harderian, extraorbital lacrimal, parotid, submaxillary, sublingual, and preputial glands) in adult (more than 5 week old) C57BL/6J male or female mice. 2x tissue volumes of 1x PBS supplemented with cOmplete EDTA-free Protease Inhibitor Cocktail tablet was used to grind tissues using disposable pestles in 1.5 ml tubes, and spun down the insoluble at 4 °C at 15,000 rpm for 30 min. 10 μl o f supernatant was served for the exposure to two CD-1 males per extract. We have similarly prepared salivary gland extracts from different strains of pups, including Balb/c, FVB, and ID.

Urine and blood:

Urine was freshly collected from adult mice of multiple strains including C57BL/6J and CD-1 in both gender. Blood was collected from some of these animals and stored at −80 °C until the date of the exposure experiments to two CD-1 males per stimulus.

Chromatographic fractions and recombinant pheromones:

10 μl of chromatographic fractions at various stages of purifications as well as recombinant pheromones were used for exposure assays. To test the stability of recombinant hemoglobins, the hemoglobin solution (4 mg/ml) was stored at room temperature (21~23 °C) for varying length of time, before exposure to mice.

Generation of Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 null mice

The Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 null alleles were produced using the homologous recombination of targeting vector containing self-excision neomycin resistance cassette using a published protocol (Chan et al., 2007). We designed the targeting vector to excise out the transmembrane domain (corresponding to the exon 6) of these V2R genes. BAC RP23–221O21 and BAC RP24–161J9 were used for the construction of Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 homology arms (long arm: 5 kb, short arm: 2 kb), respectively. Vmn2r65 targeting construct was designed to replace the exon 6 of the gene with ires-tau-EYFP and neo expression cassette, and Vmn2r88 targeting vector was design to replace a small piece of the exon 5 and entire exon 6 of the gene with ires-tau-tdTomato and neo expression cassette. The linearized targeting vectors were electroporated into V6.5 ES cells by Harvard Genome Modification Facility. ES cell clones which successfully recombined were examined by PCR spanning long arm and short arm integration sites. The ES cells in which successful targeting occurred were injected to blastocysts. The original insertion in these ES cells contained the self-excision neo cassette, which was subsequently removed in the germ line of the animal, leaving one loxP site at the end. Germ line transmissions of the mutant allele in F1 mice were confirmed by PCR genotyping. Finally, there was no expression of Vmn2r65 and Vmn2r88 genes by RNA in situ hybridization using probes specific to these genes. The F1 mice were subsequently backcrossed these null nice to C57BL/6J for 5 generations (1 male, and 4 female C57BL/6J crosses) before preparing the cohorts for behavioral testing.

Monitoring of pup-directed behaviors

Male mice (wild type, Vmr2r65 heterozygous and homozygous, Vmn2r88 heterozygous and homozygous, Vmn2r65;Vmn2r88 double mutants) were socially housed until at least 10 days before they were tested for pup-directed behaviors, at which point they were single housed. A pair of C57BL/6J pups (one males and one female, P1 to P4) was introduced to each cage, and the behaviors of resident males were video recorded using over the cage CCTV cameras under red light illuminations. When males exhibited pup-directed aggression, the pups were immediately removed, and the experiments were stopped. When the resident males exhibited parental behaviors, the behavioral monitoring was extended for another 30 min from the onset of pup retrieval. Approximately one half of wild type males neither attacked nor retrieved, even though they extensively chemoinvestigated the pups. In this scenario, the experiment was stopped after 30 min from when the pups were introduced and scored as ignoring. The marking of the individual events of social behaviors were performed using Noldus Observer XT software by independent scorers blind to the genotypes. Several behavioral parameters were monitored: chemoinvestigation with direct contact, attack latency, parental behaviors such as grooming, pup retrieval latency, and nest building. In several occasions, virgin males made nest around the pups and exhibited parental behaviors. These mice therefore did not retrieve pups, in which case, the onset of the nest building was recorded as pup retrieval latency.

Resident-Intruder assays

The intruders were prepared by swabbing 100 μl freshly sampled adult (more than 5 weeks) CD-1 male urine, to whole body, especially anogenital and neck and facial areas of castrated CD-1 males (castrated at 4 weeks). Each intruder mouse was immediately introduced into a resident cage housing wild type or double mutant males. These behaviors were video recorded for 1 hr. Social interaction events during the behavior experiments such as chemoinvestigation, tail rattling, grooming, and attacking bouts were marked by an independent scorer blind to genotypes using Noldus Observer XT software.

Mating assay

All behavioral testing was carried out within 4 hours of the onset of the dark cycle. Male sexual behavior was tested as described previously (Stowers et al., 2002; Kimchi et al., 2007). Adult virgin wild type, heterozygous and homozygous mutant littermates were individually housed for 5–7 days before conducting resident-intruder assays. On the day of the test, ovariectomized females, hormonally primed to be in estrous, were introduced into the cage and allowed to freely interact with the resident males for 60 minutes. Video recordings of social interactions were analyzed using Noldus Observer XT software. Mice were scored on standard behavioral parameters including duration of chemoinvestigation as well as latency, number and total duration of mounting, intromission and ejaculation.

Female intruders were hormonally primed as follows. Ovariectomized adult females (C57BL/6J at 8 to 9 weeks) were injected with 10 μg (in 100 μl sesame oil) of 17β-estradiol benzoate (Sigma) on 2 consecutive days preceding the test day. On the test day, mice received a subcutaneous injection of 50 μg (in 50 μl sesame oil) of progesterone, at least 6 hours before testing.

Production of silicone dummies

Rubber dummy pups were manufactured by casting of Body Double Standard Set (Smooth-On) in P1 pup mold. In this study, four different shapes were made: “dummy” as faithful pup shape, “brick” using the square cutouts of slab silicone rubber, “blob” with pup body shape without limbs or tail, and “hybrid” with brick body shape but with limbs and a tail.

Behavioral experiments with silicone dummies

A cohort of male TrpC2+/− mice was used for this experiment. We chose this genetic background since we found that a high proportion (>90 %) of these mice exhibited aggressive behaviors (Wu et al., 2014). To ensure we start the behavioral assays with homogenous pool of males, these animals were single housed at 4 months old and pre-screened the males by testing their behavior towards pups in 5 min tests at least a month prior to the dummy testing. Each male was presented with one silicone object (“dummy”, “brick”, “blob”, and “hybrid” as shown in the Figure 1A) for 10 min per day for three consecutive days. These objects were swabbed with one of the chemicals (40 μl of 1× PBS, adult CD-1 male urine, or P2~6 C57BL/6J pup salivary extracts) in a random order. These sessions were video recorded, and the male behaviors were subsequently quantified as described in the Quantification and Statistical Analysis section. The experiments were performed in double blind fashion both to the experimenter and the scorers. The same animals were tested for different shapes at least leaving one week in between to minimize potential effects of prior experiences on behavioral outcomes.

QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

RNA-seq data analysis

RNA-seq analysis was perform as shown in Figure 3E. The Illumina RNA-seq reads (in triplicates per group) were aligned using RNA-STAR to M. musculus reference genome mm10 (Ensembl). The number of fragments aligning to each gene was quantified using summarizeOverlaps from the GenomicRanges Bioconductor package. Statistical analysis of potential enrichments was performed using DESeq2 (Love et al., 2014), with multiple test correction producing a gene set with expected FDR ≤ 0.05. RNA-seq results are available at the Gene Expression Omnibus (accession number, GSE122138).

Quantification of the Egr1 activity

We used the same procedure as our previously published study (Isogai et al. 2011). To quantify the activity of different exposures, we identified Egr1+ cells in labeled VR+ neurons and calculated the fraction of co-localization in VR+ cells. At least 4 vomeronasal sections (14 or 16 μm in thickness) per animal were quantified, except for experiments involving mother’s bedding in Figure 5J, in which three sections per animal were counted for 3 animals. To perform the hierarchical screen of VRs responsive to pup stimuli in Figure 2, we initially used clade specific probes that cover all the VR clades as well as non-canonical VNO receptors such as formyl peptide receptors to narrow down the putative receptors to pup odors (N = 2 animals for each receptor clade) (Isogai et al., 2011). We then used probes that can specifically detect single to a few receptor genes to identify receptors for pup cues. The final confirmation of the receptors used three animals quantifying at least four sections. To quantify the intensity difference of the Egr1 signals observed in the activation of Vmn2r88 positive neurons by hemoglobins (Figure 5I), the intensities of Egr1 signals in Vmn2r88 expressing neurons were measured using an 87.9 μm2 circular mask, which corresponds to an approximate dimension of Vmn2r88 neurons. We also quantified pixel intensities and standard deviation of 4 to 5 neighboring non-Vmn2r88 neurons, which had background signals. Then the intensity of the Egr1 signal of each neuron was plotted as fold σ (standard deviation of the background signals). The statistical tests were performed using 2-tailed Student’s t-Test assuming unequal variances. Exact sample sizes including quantified cell numbers and the number of animals used for these tests are reported in corresponding text and figure legends.

Quantification and analysis of dummy-directed behaviors

As important criteria for the analysis of the behaviors of males towards “artificial pups,” we initially noticed that the males interact with dummies in a characteristic structure, which begins with initial approach to the dummy and subsequent chemoinvestigation with direct contact. Each interaction bout finishes with the male leaving the area. Therefore, this set of behavior was defined as one chemoinvestigative event and classified them as non-aggressive or aggressive. Non-aggressive bouts only had sniffing and grooming, while aggressive bouts contained biting interleaving with chemoinvestigation. Using Noldus Observer XT software the duration and frequency of aggressive and total (i.e., aggressive plus non-aggressive) chemoinvestigative bouts were subsequently quantified by a human observer blind to the nature of chemosignals swabbed on the dummies. The sliding window analysis was performed using 2 min as the window. The statistical tests were performed using paired t-Test using R and GraphPad Prism. The number of animals used is described in the corresponding figure legends.

Analysis of receptor knockout mice

The distribution of the males that attacked or parent, or ignored in different genotypes were analyzed using Fisher’s Exact Test, as performed previously (Wu et al. 2015). The significance of quantified behavioral parameters in Supplemental figure 6 was analyzed by two-tailed t-Test assuming unequal variance. The number of animals in each test is indicated in the corresponding text and figure legends.

DATA AND SOFTWARE AVAILABILITY

The raw and processed RNA-seq data in this study have been deposited in the Gene Expression Omnibus (GEO:GSE122138, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE122138).

Supplementary Material

10

Supplemental video 1. Related to Figure 1

Video showing virgin male interactions with a pup dummy, which was previously scented by co-housing with a mother and pups.

Download video file (42MB, mp4)
8. Supplemental figure 6 (in relation to the figure 6): Quantification of social behaviors in receptor knockout mice.

(A) Analysis of pup-directed behaviors of mutants including the attack latency of the aggressive animals (Vmn2r65+/+, N = 19; Vmn2r65+/−, N = 20; Vmn2r65−/−, N = 13; Vmn2r88+/+, N = 17; Vmn2r88+/−, N = 15; Vmn2r88−/−, N = 17; dKO, N = 5), the total chemoinvestigative events of ignoring animals, (Vmn2r65+/+, N = 16; Vmn2r65+/−, N = 9; Vmn2r65−/−, N = 12; Vmn2r88+/+, N = 13; Vmn2r88+/−, N = 12; Vmn2r88−/−, N = 7; dKO, N =18), and a repertoire of parental behavior (Vmn2r65+/+, N = 1; Vmn2r65+/−, N = 1; Vmn2r65−/−, N = 10; Vmn2r88+/+, N = 1; Vmn2r88+/−, N = 4; Vmn2r88−/−, N = 5; dKO, N = 7). Comparisons yield no statistically significant difference between genotypes by t-Test.

(B) The analysis of mating behavior in Vmn2r65−/− males (Vmn2r65+/+ or Vmn2r65+/−, N = 13; Vmn2r65−/−, N = 14). Vmn2r65−/− virgin males were presented with hormonally primed estrus females and individual behaviors representative of mating response were quantified.

(C) Resident intruder assay with Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− virgin males. The table shows the overall counts of the attacked animals in 1 hr behavioral bout (p = 0.20 by Fisher Exact Test). *p < 0.05 by t-Test. The graphs show the analysis of territorial aggression of Vmn2r65−/−Vmn2r88−/− males (WT males, N = 13; Vmn2r65−/−Vmn2r88−/−, N = 20). Several parameters such as the total time in chemoinvestigation, grooming and attack latency exhibited statistically significant difference. *p < 0.05 by Student’s t-Test.

9

Supplemental figure 7 (in relation to Figure 7): Summary of the sensory requirements for pup-directed aggression by virgin males.

11

Supplemental video 2. Related to Figure 1

Video showing virgin male interactions with a pup dummy swabbed with pup salivary extracts.

Download video file (5.1MB, mp4)
12

Supplemental video 3. Related to Figure 1

Video (0.25× real time) captured by a high-speed camera showing the initial investigation of a mouse pup by a virgin male.

Download video file (48.2MB, mp4)
2

Supplemental table 1: The target sequences of RNA FISH probes (in Excel format)

3

Supplemental figure 1 (in relation to Figure 1):

(A-D) Raster plots of behavioral events by virgin males exposed to various types of silicone shapes swabbed with pup salivary extract, PBS, or male urine. The aggressive and non-aggressive bouts are marked by red and blue boxes, respectively. Below each raster plot is a sliding window analysis (2 min), using the same procedure as in Figure 1I. N = 15.

4. Supplemental figure 2 (in relation to the figure 2): Responses of Vmn2r122/123 and Vmn2r88 to various social stimuli.

(A) Egr1 is robustly induced in VNO neurons expressing Vmn2r122/123 in virgin males exposed to male C57BL/6J submaxillary gland extracts (82.2 ± 6.0% of Vmn2r122/123 cells are Egr1+; Error in SEM; 38 Vmn2r122/123+ neurons from 2 animals examined).

(B) Response of Vmn2r88 neurons to pups in virgin females exhibiting maternal behaviors. 78.9 ± 6.2% of Vmn2r88 neurons overlap with Egr1 (245 Vmn2r88 neurons examined, N = 3 animals). Arrow marks the overlap of Egr1 and VR signals.

The scale bar represents 100 μm.

5. Supplemental figure 3 (in relation to the Figure 3): V2R receptors detecting pups are sex and strain independent.

(A-D) In situ hybridization with probes of Egr1 and vomeronasal receptors on VNOs of virgin CD-1 males exposed to salivary gland extracts of male or female pups from 4 different strains (n =2 animals per stimulus).

(E) Quantification of in situ hybridization signals. The percentage of VR+ neurons that also co-express Egr1 was quantified in the above experiments. The errors are in SEM, and total numbers of VR+ neurons quantified are indicated in parentheses.

6. Supplemental figure 4 (in relation to the figure 4): Additional data for the identification of Smgc as a ligand for Vmn2r65.

(A) List of proteins identified by mass spectrometry that are exclusively enriched in ion exchange column fractions derived from female salivary gland.

(B) Smgc peptides identified by mass spectrometry of the partially fractionated preparation of pup salivary glands.

7. Supplemental figure 5 (in relation to the figure 5): Additional data for the identification of hemoglobins as ligands for Vmn2r88.

(A) List of proteins identified by mass spectrometry in purified active fraction derived from C57BL/6J male salivary gland extracts. Hemoglobin Hbb-bt (in this diagram denoted as Hbbt1, per Uniprot nomenclature) emerges as a top candidate.

(B) The peak activity fraction of Poros HQ chromatography is red.

(C) Silver stained gel of the peak activity fraction derived from pup salivary gland extracts showed a unique 14 kDa band.

(D) In situ hybridization of Vmn2r88 and Egr1 in the VNO of animals exposed to the peak fraction shown in (C).

(E) Peptides corresponding to hemoglobins identified by mass spectrometry analysis of the purified pup fractions. This analysis confirmed that Hbb-bt (or Hbbt1) is a top candidate.

(F) Gel filtration chromatography of recombinant hemoglobin Hbb-bt/Hba-a1. Purified recombinant hemoglobin appeared predominantly as a dimer rather than tetramer. Endogenous hemoglobin tetramer in C57BL/J male blood extracts has a significantly shorter retention time than the major recombinant hemoglobin peak. Note that the Hbaa1 subunit in the recombinant hemoglobin is 11 amino acids longer than the native protein, resulting in a shorter retention time.

(G) Quantitative analysis of the retention times of hemoglobins in gel filtration chromatography. The table shows the mean retention time of the tetramer and dimer peaks (shown as arrows in F) (N = 3 technical replicates).

Highlights.

  • Reconstituted pup shape and chemosignals trigger aggression by virgin males

  • Repertoire of 7 VNO receptors activated by pups is also stimulated by adult cues

  • Deletion of receptors to salivary protein and hemoglobin shows role in pup attack

  • Complex recognition involves pup’s shape and chemosignals from infants and parents

Acknowledgement

We thank members of the Dulac lab for critical reading of the manuscript, Drs. J. Sanes, M. Yamagata, X. Duan, and P. Liu for recombineering plasmids, the Harvard Genome Modification Facility for generating mouse mutants, the Harvard Proteomics Facility for mass spectrometry, Harvard FAS Bauer Core for Illumina sequencing. We thank Drs. R. Irizarry for assistance with statistics, J. Moore for helping silicone pup fabrication, and D. Lessing for experimental help. We are particularly grateful for S. Sullivan’s expert assistance with animal experiments and procedures. We also thank Dr. R. Axel for advice on the manuscript. M.I.L. is supported by NIH grants CA142538–08 and 5T32CA009337–35, Y.I. by a Charles A. King Trust fellowship and funds from the Gatsby Charitable Foundation and Wellcome Trust. C.D. is supported by HHMI and by NIH grants R01DC013087, R01DC003903, and R01HD082131.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Declaration of interests

The authors declare no competing interests associated with this study.

References

  1. Beach FA (1942a). Analysis of factors involved in the arousal, maintenance and manifestation of sexual excitement in male animals. Psychosomatic Medicine 4, 173–198. [Google Scholar]
  2. Beach FA (1942b). Analysis of the stimuli adequate to elicit mating behavior in the sexually inexperienced rat. Journal of Comparative Psychology 33, 163–207. [Google Scholar]
  3. Beach FA, and Jaynes J (1956). Studies of Maternal Retrieving in Rats. III. Sensory Cues Involved in the Lactating Female’s Response To Her Young. Behaviour 10, 104–124. [Google Scholar]
  4. Chamero P, Marton TF, Logan DW, Flanagan K, Cruz JR, Saghatelian A, Cravatt BF, and Stowers L (2007). Identification of protein pheromones that promote aggressive behaviour. Nature 450, 899–902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chan W, Costantino N, Li R, Lee SC, Su Q, Melvin D, Court DL, and Liu P (2007). A recombineering based approach for high-throughput conditional knockout targeting vector construction. Nucleic Acids Res 35, e64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen P, and Hong W (2018). Neural Circuit Mechanisms of Social Behavior. Neuron 98, 16–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Ferrero DM, Moeller LM, Osakada T, Horio N, Li Q, Roy DS, Cichy A, Spehr M, Touhara K, and Liberles SD (2013). A juvenile mouse pheromone inhibits sexual behaviour through the vomeronasal system. Nature 502, 368–371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Fraser EJ, and Shah NM (2014). Complex chemosensory control of female reproductive behaviors. PLoS One 9, e90368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Fu X, Yan Y, Xu PS, Geerlof-Vidavsky I, Chong W, Gross ML, and Holy TE (2015). A Molecular Code for Identity in the Vomeronasal System. Cell 163, 313–323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gomes I, Dale CS, Casten K, Geigner MA, Gozzo FC, Ferro ES, Heimann AS, and Devi LA (2010). Hemoglobin-derived peptides as novel type of bioactive signaling molecules. AAPS J 12, 658–669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Haga S, Hattori T, Sato T, Sato K, Matsuda S, Kobayakawa R, Sakano H, Yoshihara Y, Kikusui T, and Touhara K (2010). The male mouse pheromone ESP1 enhances female sexual receptive behaviour through a specific vomeronasal receptor. Nature 466, 118–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hausfater G, and Hrdy SB (1984). Infanticide : comparative and evolutionary perspectives (New York: Aldine Pub. Co.). [Google Scholar]
  13. Hempe JM, Ory-Ascani J, and Hsia D (2007). Genetic variation in mouse beta globin cysteine content modifies glutathione metabolism: implications for the use of mouse models. Exp Biol Med (Maywood) 232, 437–444. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hrdy SB (1974). Male-male competition and infanticide among the langurs (Presbytis entellus) of Abu, Rajasthan. Folia Primatol (Basel) 22, 19–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Isogai Y, Si S, Pont-Lezica L, Tan T, Kapoor V, Murthy VN, and Dulac C (2011). Molecular organization of vomeronasal chemoreception. Nature 478, 241–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kaur AW, Ackels T, Kuo TH, Cichy A, Dey S, Hays C, Kateri M, Logan DW, Marton TF, Spehr M, et al. (2014). Murine pheromone proteins constitute a context-dependent combinatorial code governing multiple social behaviors. Cell 157, 676–688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kimchi T, Xu J, and Dulac C (2007). A functional circuit underlying male sexual behaviour in the female mouse brain. Nature 448, 1009–1014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Knight ZA, Tan K, Birsoy K, Schmidt S, Garrison JL, Wysocki RW, Emiliano A, Ekstrand MI, and Friedman JM (2012). Molecular profiling of activated neurons by phosphorylated ribosome capture. Cell 151, 1126–1137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Liberles SD (2014). Mammalian pheromones. Annu Rev Physiol 76, 151–175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Love MI, Huber W, and Anders S (2014). Moderated estimation of fold change and dispersion for RNA-seq data with DESeq2. Genome Biol 15, 550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lukas D, and Huchard E (2014). Sexual conflict. The evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian societies. Science 346, 841–844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Marder E (2012). Neuromodulation of neuronal circuits: back to the future. Neuron 76, 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Marlin BJ, Mitre M, D’Amour JA, Chao MV, and Froemke RC (2015). Oxytocin enables maternal behaviour by balancing cortical inhibition. Nature 520, 499–504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Matsunami H, and Buck LB (1997). A multigene family encoding a diverse array of putative pheromone receptors in mammals. Cell 90, 775–784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. McCarthy MM, and vom Saal FS (1985). The influence of reproductive state on infanticide by wild female house mice (Mus musculus). Physiol Behav 35, 843–849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. McCarthy MM, and Vom Saal FS (1986). Inhibition of infanticide after mating by wild male house mice. Physiol Behav 36, 203–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mennella JA, and Moltz H (1988). Infanticide in the male rat: the role of the vomeronasal organ. Physiol Behav 42, 303–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Raabe BM, Artwohl JE, Purcell JE, Lovaglio J, and Fortman JD (2011). Effects of weekly blood collection in C57BL/6 mice. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 50, 680–685. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Ryba NJ, and Tirindelli R (1997). A new multigene family of putative pheromone receptors. Neuron 19, 371–379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Stern JM (1990). MULTISENSORY REGULATION OF MATERNAL-BEHAVIOR AND MASCULINE SEXUAL-BEHAVIOR - A REVISED VIEW. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 14, 183–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Stowers L, Holy TE, Meister M, Dulac C, and Koentges G (2002). Loss of sex discrimination and male-male aggression in mice deficient for TRP2. Science 295, 1493–1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Sugiyama Y (1965). On the social change of hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) in their natural condition. Primates 6, 381–418. [Google Scholar]
  33. Tachikawa KS, Yoshihara Y, and Kuroda KO (2013). Behavioral transition from attack to parenting in male mice: a crucial role of the vomeronasal system. J Neurosci 33, 5120–5126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Tinbergen N (1969). The study of instinct, 1st edn (Oxford,: Clarendon P.). [Google Scholar]
  35. Weaver S, Comer MB, Jahn CL, Hutchison CA 3rd, and Edgell MH (1981). The adult beta-globin genes of the “single” type mouse C57BL. Cell 24, 403–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Wu Z, Autry AE, Bergan JF, Watabe-Uchida M, and Dulac CG (2014). Galanin neurons in the medial preoptic area govern parental behaviour. Nature 509, 325–330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Zinzen KM, Hand AR, Yankova M, Ball WD, and Mirels L (2004). Molecular cloning and characterization of the neonatal rat and mouse submandibular gland protein SMGC. Gene 334, 23–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

10

Supplemental video 1. Related to Figure 1

Video showing virgin male interactions with a pup dummy, which was previously scented by co-housing with a mother and pups.

Download video file (42MB, mp4)
8. Supplemental figure 6 (in relation to the figure 6): Quantification of social behaviors in receptor knockout mice.

(A) Analysis of pup-directed behaviors of mutants including the attack latency of the aggressive animals (Vmn2r65+/+, N = 19; Vmn2r65+/−, N = 20; Vmn2r65−/−, N = 13; Vmn2r88+/+, N = 17; Vmn2r88+/−, N = 15; Vmn2r88−/−, N = 17; dKO, N = 5), the total chemoinvestigative events of ignoring animals, (Vmn2r65+/+, N = 16; Vmn2r65+/−, N = 9; Vmn2r65−/−, N = 12; Vmn2r88+/+, N = 13; Vmn2r88+/−, N = 12; Vmn2r88−/−, N = 7; dKO, N =18), and a repertoire of parental behavior (Vmn2r65+/+, N = 1; Vmn2r65+/−, N = 1; Vmn2r65−/−, N = 10; Vmn2r88+/+, N = 1; Vmn2r88+/−, N = 4; Vmn2r88−/−, N = 5; dKO, N = 7). Comparisons yield no statistically significant difference between genotypes by t-Test.

(B) The analysis of mating behavior in Vmn2r65−/− males (Vmn2r65+/+ or Vmn2r65+/−, N = 13; Vmn2r65−/−, N = 14). Vmn2r65−/− virgin males were presented with hormonally primed estrus females and individual behaviors representative of mating response were quantified.

(C) Resident intruder assay with Vmn2r65−/−;Vmn2r88−/− virgin males. The table shows the overall counts of the attacked animals in 1 hr behavioral bout (p = 0.20 by Fisher Exact Test). *p < 0.05 by t-Test. The graphs show the analysis of territorial aggression of Vmn2r65−/−Vmn2r88−/− males (WT males, N = 13; Vmn2r65−/−Vmn2r88−/−, N = 20). Several parameters such as the total time in chemoinvestigation, grooming and attack latency exhibited statistically significant difference. *p < 0.05 by Student’s t-Test.

9

Supplemental figure 7 (in relation to Figure 7): Summary of the sensory requirements for pup-directed aggression by virgin males.

11

Supplemental video 2. Related to Figure 1

Video showing virgin male interactions with a pup dummy swabbed with pup salivary extracts.

Download video file (5.1MB, mp4)
12

Supplemental video 3. Related to Figure 1

Video (0.25× real time) captured by a high-speed camera showing the initial investigation of a mouse pup by a virgin male.

Download video file (48.2MB, mp4)
2

Supplemental table 1: The target sequences of RNA FISH probes (in Excel format)

3

Supplemental figure 1 (in relation to Figure 1):

(A-D) Raster plots of behavioral events by virgin males exposed to various types of silicone shapes swabbed with pup salivary extract, PBS, or male urine. The aggressive and non-aggressive bouts are marked by red and blue boxes, respectively. Below each raster plot is a sliding window analysis (2 min), using the same procedure as in Figure 1I. N = 15.

4. Supplemental figure 2 (in relation to the figure 2): Responses of Vmn2r122/123 and Vmn2r88 to various social stimuli.

(A) Egr1 is robustly induced in VNO neurons expressing Vmn2r122/123 in virgin males exposed to male C57BL/6J submaxillary gland extracts (82.2 ± 6.0% of Vmn2r122/123 cells are Egr1+; Error in SEM; 38 Vmn2r122/123+ neurons from 2 animals examined).

(B) Response of Vmn2r88 neurons to pups in virgin females exhibiting maternal behaviors. 78.9 ± 6.2% of Vmn2r88 neurons overlap with Egr1 (245 Vmn2r88 neurons examined, N = 3 animals). Arrow marks the overlap of Egr1 and VR signals.

The scale bar represents 100 μm.

5. Supplemental figure 3 (in relation to the Figure 3): V2R receptors detecting pups are sex and strain independent.

(A-D) In situ hybridization with probes of Egr1 and vomeronasal receptors on VNOs of virgin CD-1 males exposed to salivary gland extracts of male or female pups from 4 different strains (n =2 animals per stimulus).

(E) Quantification of in situ hybridization signals. The percentage of VR+ neurons that also co-express Egr1 was quantified in the above experiments. The errors are in SEM, and total numbers of VR+ neurons quantified are indicated in parentheses.

6. Supplemental figure 4 (in relation to the figure 4): Additional data for the identification of Smgc as a ligand for Vmn2r65.

(A) List of proteins identified by mass spectrometry that are exclusively enriched in ion exchange column fractions derived from female salivary gland.

(B) Smgc peptides identified by mass spectrometry of the partially fractionated preparation of pup salivary glands.

7. Supplemental figure 5 (in relation to the figure 5): Additional data for the identification of hemoglobins as ligands for Vmn2r88.

(A) List of proteins identified by mass spectrometry in purified active fraction derived from C57BL/6J male salivary gland extracts. Hemoglobin Hbb-bt (in this diagram denoted as Hbbt1, per Uniprot nomenclature) emerges as a top candidate.

(B) The peak activity fraction of Poros HQ chromatography is red.

(C) Silver stained gel of the peak activity fraction derived from pup salivary gland extracts showed a unique 14 kDa band.

(D) In situ hybridization of Vmn2r88 and Egr1 in the VNO of animals exposed to the peak fraction shown in (C).

(E) Peptides corresponding to hemoglobins identified by mass spectrometry analysis of the purified pup fractions. This analysis confirmed that Hbb-bt (or Hbbt1) is a top candidate.

(F) Gel filtration chromatography of recombinant hemoglobin Hbb-bt/Hba-a1. Purified recombinant hemoglobin appeared predominantly as a dimer rather than tetramer. Endogenous hemoglobin tetramer in C57BL/J male blood extracts has a significantly shorter retention time than the major recombinant hemoglobin peak. Note that the Hbaa1 subunit in the recombinant hemoglobin is 11 amino acids longer than the native protein, resulting in a shorter retention time.

(G) Quantitative analysis of the retention times of hemoglobins in gel filtration chromatography. The table shows the mean retention time of the tetramer and dimer peaks (shown as arrows in F) (N = 3 technical replicates).

Data Availability Statement

The raw and processed RNA-seq data in this study have been deposited in the Gene Expression Omnibus (GEO:GSE122138, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE122138).

RESOURCES