Abstract
(1) Background: Parental feeding practices are related to child body mass index (BMI, kg/m2) and seem to be a consequence rather than cause of child BMI, but research so far is limited. Stability and continuity of feeding practices, probably explaining changes in food intake and child BMI, remain to be poorly examined. (2) Methods: Feeding practices (i.e., restriction, food as reward, pressure to eat, monitoring) assessed via the Child Feeding Questionnaire, child age, standardized BMI (zBMI), and socio-economic status were measured annually at multiple visits (range 2–8) in a population-based longitudinal cohort study of 1512 parents with their children aged 2 to 12 years. Stability, continuity, and bi-directionality of feeding practices and child zBMI were calculated using correlation coefficients, paired t tests, and cross-lagged panels, respectively. (3) Results: Feeding practices and child zBMI showed moderate to high stability. While continuity was high for restriction, minor temporal changes were observed for other feeding practices and child zBMI. Cross-lags indicated that child zBMI predicted restriction, pressure to eat, and monitoring, while food-rewarding predicted child zBMI only minorly. (4) Conclusions: Parents seem to adapt feeding practices to child zBMI with the exception of food-rewarding.
Keywords: feeding practices, body mass index, child weight, children, stability, bi-directional
1. Introduction
Overweight and obesity in childhood and adolescence are an increasing public health concern [1,2,3]. While in 1980 16.9% of boys and 16.2% of girls were overweight or obese, international prevalence rates rose to 23.8% and 22.6% in 2013, respectively [4]. Overweight and obesity in childhood are predictive of overweight and obesity in adulthood and the onset of related physical (e.g., cardiovascular syndrome, metabolic syndrome) and psychological comorbidities (e.g., depressive disorders, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; [5,6,7]). Fundamentally, overweight and obesity are caused by an imbalance between energy intake and expenditure [8], but their etiologies are complex [1,9]. Notably, the increasing prevalence of overweight and obesity suggests that environmental and genetic factors are involved in the development of childhood overweight and obesity. A change in these environmental factors could promote a reduction in the obesity epidemic [10]. One of the potential environmental factors influencing energy intake in children are parental feeding practices [11], which include parental beliefs, attitudes, and practices concerning child feeding [12].
Increasing evidence summarized in a systematic review by Shloim et al. shows that parental feeding practices were related to child weight status, mostly based on cross-sectional evidence [11]. In this context, restrictive feeding (i.e., parental restriction of their child’s access to food) was found to strengthen the child’s preference for the restricted food [13] and to be cross-sectionally related to higher standardized child body mass index (zBMI; [11]). Using food as reward (i.e., rewarding positive child behavior with food) was found to be positively associated with child zBMI in a cross-sectional study [14]. Another key parental feeding practice, pressure to eat (i.e., parental tendency to force their children to eat more), was found to create negative feelings to the pressured foods [13] and was negatively associated with child zBMI in cross-sectional studies [11]. For the association between parental monitoring of child’s food intake (i.e., parental oversight of their child’s eating) and child zBMI, cross-sectional studies provided ambiguous findings, mostly demonstrating lack of associations [15,16].
Concerning longitudinal findings, evidence is weaker compared to cross-sectional findings or inconsistent yet [11]. Specifically, restrictive feeding showed no prospective effects on child zBMI in two longitudinal studies in children aged 5 to 11 years [15,16], except in a study of n = 204 parents and their children indicating restrictive feeding assessed between the ages of 5 and 6 years at baseline to predict lower child zBMI at a 3-year follow-up [17]. Using food as reward in children aged 2 years at baseline was found to be prospectively associated with greater child zBMI gain 1 year later [18]. Further results on pressure to eat and child zBMI indicated bi-directionality, with a prospective relationship between greater pressure to eat and lower child zBMI in 1- to 2- and 5- to 7-year-old children [19,20] and a prospective association between lower child zBMI and higher use of pressure to eat between the ages of 7 and 11 [16]. The few existing evidence on the associations between parental monitoring and child zBMI was inconsistent, indicating a lack of association in 7- to 11-year-old children [16] or protective effects of monitoring in 5-year-old children on child zBMI at 7 years of age [15].
It is important to note that previous studies are predominantly cross-sectional in design [11], not allowing any conclusions whether parental feeding practices may be a cause or consequence of child zBMI. In addition, the few longitudinal studies carried out to date have concentrated mainly on predicting child zBMI by parental feeding practices, not considering the assumption that parental feeding practices can be a consequence of child weight status, too [21,22]. In this context, first studies analyzed the bi-directional effects of parental controlling feeding practices (i.e., restrictive feeding, pressure to eat, and monitoring) and objectively measured child zBMI in different European samples of n = 213 7- to 9-year-old children with a 3-year follow-up [16], n = 526 6- to 11-year-old children with a 10-month follow-up [21], n = 3708 4-year-old children with a 3-year follow-up [23], n = 4166 2-year-old children with two 2-year follow-ups [24], and n = 4689 4-year-old children with a 6-year follow-up [25]. Consistently, these studies suggested that controlling feeding practices are a consequence rather than a cause of child zBMI [16,21,23,24,25]. However, research that examines the entire childhood from early to middle childhood is still lacking.
Beyond the observation that parental feeding practices are associated with child zBMI [11] and that child weight status from the age of 5 years on is relatively stable and continuous into childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood [26,27,28,29], little is known about the stability (consistency of individual levels over time) and continuity (consistency of group levels over time) of parental feeding practices across childhood [30]. First evidence from relatively small samples (n = 31–183; [19,30,31,32,33]) suggested that there was high stability and continuity in parental controlling feeding practices assessed via the Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ; [12]), but these studies only examined narrow age ranges: 1–2 years [32], 3–4 years [31,33], 2–5 years [30], and 5–7 years [19]. A study in older children from a British population, however, indicated decreasing levels of parental controlling feeding practices in their children aged between 7 and 11 years [16], leaving the question whether the continuity of parental feeding practices changes from early to middle childhood.
Therefore, the aim of this study was to determine the stability and continuity of parental feeding practices and child zBMI, and their bi-directional associations in a large longitudinal population-based sample of 2- to 12-year-old German children. According to previous research mentioned above, it was hypothesized that (1) parental restrictive feeding, food as reward, pressure to eat, monitoring, and child zBMI are relatively stable and continuous during childhood, (2) restrictive feeding and child zBMI show a bi-directionally positive prospective association, (3) food as reward positively predicts child zBMI, while there is no effect of child zBMI on food as reward, (4) pressure to eat and child zBMI show a bi-directionally negative prospective association, and (5) child zBMI and parental monitoring are not related to each other over time.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants and Procedures
Study data were provided from the ‘Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases (LIFE) Child’ study (‘LIFE Child Study’), which is a prospective, longitudinal population-based cohort study collecting data from fetal life until adulthood. In the ‘LIFE Child Study’, participants are recruited without further exclusion criteria at different time points, with recruitment age ranging between the 24th week of gestation and 16 years of child age, and reassessed at multiple follow-ups. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig, Germany (reg. no. 264-10-19042010) and conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration [34]. Advertisement in kindergartens, schools, health care practices (e.g., in hospitals, clinics, public health centers, and doctors’ surgeries), and by media (e.g., internet, radio, and television) was used for recruitment. At the time of the current study data of 2723 parent-child dyads were available. All parents were informed about the study in detail, data use, potential risks of participation, and the right to withdraw from the study without explanations or adverse consequences at any time, and signed informed written consent forms. All parents and children participated in comprehensive assessments (e.g., clinical examinations, questionnaires, and interviews) conducted by well-trained assessors (e.g., physicians, nurses, psychologists, and nutritional scientists) in a research center at the University Hospital of Leipzig at different time points. Monetary (≤ 20 Euro per child) and some further incentives (e.g., items with the study logo, feedback on results) served as a compensation for participation (for detailed information, see [35,36]). Eligible for the current study were n = 1512 child-parent dyads with one follow-up assessment at minimum assessed 10–14 months after baseline and valid data on the CFQ, standardized child BMI, child age, child sex, and baseline socio-economic status. Assessment was carried out at least twice in n = 328 children at age 2 to 3, n = 366 at age 3 to 4, n = 299 at age 4 to 5, n = 358 at age 5 to 6, n = 307 at age 6 to 7, n = 401 at age 7 to 8, n = 372 at age 8 to 9, n = 416 at age 9 to 10, n = 360 at age 10 to 11, and n = 363 at age 11 to 12.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ)
The CFQ is a 31-item questionnaire used to assess three factors on parents’ attitudes and practices on child feeding and four factors on beliefs and concerns on child feeding and weight. For the current study, only the factors on parental feeding practices were used. The three factors on parental feeding practices from the original version by Birch et al. [12] contain restrictive feeding (eight items), pressure to eat (four items), and monitoring (three items). As recent psychometric analyses suggested the establishment of a fourth subscale, food as reward, consisting of two items of the original restriction subscale [37,38,39], restrictive feeding (six items) and food as reward (two items) were considered as separate parental feeding practices in the present study. All CFQ items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale recording agreement (1 = disagree to 5 = agree) or frequency (1 = never to 5 = always), with higher scores indicating greater controlling feeding practices and food-rewarding. Mean scores were used to quantify each of the four factors. The CFQ was gathered at all annual follow-up visits. The CFQ showed acceptable to excellent internal consistency of the subscales (Cronbach’s α = 0.71–0.91) in previous research using a subsample of this study’s sample (n = 982 mothers and their 2- to 13-year-old children; [37]).
2.2.2. Child Characteristics
Children’s height and weight were measured by a stadiometer ‘Dr. Keller I’ (Längenmesstechnik Limbach GmbH, Limbach-Oberfrohna, Germany) and the calibrated scale ‘Seca 701’ (Seca Gmbh & Co. KG, Hamburg, Germany) at all visits. BMI (kg/m2) was transformed into BMI z scores (zBMI) according to sex- and age-specific references published by Kromeyer-Hauschild et al. [40]. Child sex and age were assessed by parent-report at baseline.
2.2.3. Socio-Economic Status (SES)
The family socio-economic status (SES) was evaluated at all follow-up visits using the Winkler Index [41,42], ranging from 3 to 21. Based on income, education, and occupation families were classified as having low (3–8), medium (9–14), or high (15–21) SES.
2.3. Statistical Analyses
First, CFQ items were analyzed for missing values. Missing values were computed by the participant’s mean of the subscale in case of ≤ 25% missing values per subscale. Otherwise the participant was excluded from the analyses. Shapiro–Wilks tests of normal distribution were calculated for all variables. Based on outlier analyses, data which were three standard deviations under or above the mean of each variable were eliminated.
Stability of parental feeding practices and child zBMI was examined using Pearson product-moment or Spearman rank order correlations for normally or non-normally distributed data, respectively. Additionally, paired t tests and post-hoc effect size analyses using Cohen’s d were examined to analyze continuity of parental feeding practices and zBMI at the different visits and their follow-up. Using the software package G*Power (Version 3.1.9.2; [43]), a post-hoc power analysis showed that the estimated statistical power (1–β) for the paired t tests (n = 299–416, two-tailed α = 0.05) was 1.00 for detecting a medium (Cohen’s d = 0.5) or large effect (Cohen’s d = 0.8).
Cross-lagged panel design using structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test the bi-directional associations between each parental feeding practice (i.e., restrictive feeding, food as reward, pressure to eat, and monitoring) and child zBMI. SEM was run as path analysis, including correlations to examine the cross-sectional associations within parental feeding practices and between each parental feeding practice and child zBMI, and linear regressions to analyze longitudinal associations within both variables, and the cross-lags between both variables from 2 to 11 years of age at baseline and the mean 1-year follow-up. In accordance with the literature, the models were adjusted for child sex and SES [23,24]. Figure 1 visualizes the cross-lagged panel analysis. Standardized βs were used to determine the relative strength of the examined associations. Model fit indices were calculated, with cut-offs in parentheses indicating good fit: χ2 (p > 0.05), root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA ≤ 0.05), comparative fit index (CFI ≥ 0.95), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI ≥ 0.95; [44]), and minimum discrepancy divided by its degrees of freedom (CMIN/df < 2).
A two-tailed p < 0.05 was set for statistical significance. Except for the SEM, which was calculated using Amos (Version 20.0; IBM SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA), statistical analyses were run using SPSS for Windows (Version 24.0; IBM SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).
3. Results
3.1. Sample Characteristics
Within the total sample, all children (n = 1512) were assessed with multiple visits, ranging between two and eight visits (M = 3.74, SD = 1.40). Follow-up was conducted with a mean lag of 11.55 ± 0.79 months, ranging between 10 and 14 months. Age-specific sample characteristics are shown in Table 1.
Table 1.
Years of Age | ||||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 to 3 (n = 328) |
3 to 4 (n = 366) |
4 to 5 (n = 299) |
5 to 6 (n = 358) |
6 to 7 (n = 307) |
7 to 8 (n = 401) |
8 to 9 (n = 372) |
9 to 10 (n = 416) |
10 to 11 (n = 360) |
11 to 12 (n = 363) |
|||||||||||
Children | ||||||||||||||||||||
Sex % girls/boys |
49.1/50.9 | 48.6/51.4 | 48.2/51.8 | 48.0/52.0 | 50.2/49.8 | 49.1/50.9 | 44.9/55.1 | 44.0/56.0 | 43.6/56.4 | 46.0/54.0 | ||||||||||
Age | 2.04 ± 0.16 | 3.05 ± 0.17 | 3.02 ± 0.21 | 4.03 ± 0.21 | 4.04 ± 0.26 | 5.04 ± 0.26 | 4.99 ± 0.27 | 5.99 ± 0.27 | 5.97 ± 0.28 | 6.98 ± 0.29 | 6.97 ± 0.29 | 7.97 ± 0.28 | 7.99 ± 0.28 | 9.00 ± 0.28 | 8.99 ± 0.27 | 10.00 ± 0.27 | 9.98 ± 0.28 | 10.99 ± 0.28 | 11.01 ± 0.29 | 12.02 ± 0.29 |
zBMI | 0.24 ± 0.81 | 0.23 ± 0.74 | 0.20 ± 0.74 | 0.03 ± 0.73 | −0.01 ± 0.77 | −0.11 ± 0.79 | −0.12 ± 0.82 | −0.15 ± 0.87 | −0.14 ± 0.84 | −0.15 ± 0.85 | −0.14 ± 0.90 | −0.18 ± 0.91 | −0.07 ± 0.97 | −0.07 ± 1.01 | 0.01 ± 1.02 | 0.05 ± 1.02 | 0.08 ± 1.02 | 0.09 ± 1.05 | 0.18 ± 1.12 | 0.22 ± 1.14 |
zBMI % | ||||||||||||||||||||
underweight | 1.2 | 0.6 | 0.3 | 0.3 | 0.7 | 0.3 | 0.8 | 1.1 | 0.7 | 0.3 | 1.0 | 1.0 | 0.3 | 1.1 | 1.2 | 1.7 | 0.8 | 2.2 | 1.7 | 1.1 |
normal weight | 89.9 | 92.1 | 92.3 | 94.5 | 95.3 | 94.7 | 92.7 | 92.2 | 93.8 | 92.5 | 92.3 | 92.3 | 90.9 | 88.4 | 86.8 | 85.8 | 86.7 | 83.3 | 80.7 | 81.0 |
overweight | 6.7 | 6.4 | 6.3 | 4.4 | 3.7 | 4.7 | 5.3 | 3.6 | 2.9 | 4.6 | 3.0 | 2.7 | 3.2 | 4.3 | 4.6 | 5.5 | 4.7 | 6.4 | 7.4 | 7.4 |
obesity | 2.1 | 0.9 | 1.1 | 0.8 | 0.3 | 0.3 | 1.1 | 3.1 | 2.6 | 2.6 | 3.7 | 4.0 | 5.6 | 6.2 | 7.5 | 7.0 | 7.8 | 8.1 | 10.2 | 10.5 |
SES | 13.79 ± 3.02 | 13.89 ± 3.13 | 14.24 ± 3.12 | 14.08 ± 3.13 | 13.96 ± 3.18 | 13.83 ± 3.28 | 13.62 ± 3.21 | 13.68 ± 3.39 | 13.42 ± 3.23 | 13.63 ± 3.32 | ||||||||||
SES % | ||||||||||||||||||||
low | 4.6 | 7.1 | 5.0 | 5.3 | 5.2 | 7.5 | 8.3 | 6.7 | 6.7 | 8.8 | ||||||||||
medium | 51.8 | 44.0 | 40.5 | 45.0 | 45.0 | 42.9 | 45.2 | 48.3 | 51.9 | 47.4 | ||||||||||
high | 43.6 | 48.9 | 54.5 | 49.7 | 49.8 | 49.6 | 46.5 | 45.0 | 41.4 | 43.8 | ||||||||||
CFQ | ||||||||||||||||||||
RST | 2.67 ± 0.84 | 2.70 ± 0.79 | 2.70 ± 0.82 | 2.60 ± 0.83 | 2.54 ± 0.80 | 2.49 ± 0.80 | 2.51 ± 0.83 | 2.42 ± 0.82 | 2.35 ± 0.82 | 2.33 ± 0.84 | 2.31 ± 0.85 | 2.23 ± 0.85 | 2.32 ± 0.85 | 2.31 ± 0.84 | 2.35 ± 0.89 | 2.29 ± 0.93 | 2.32 ± 0.90 | 2.29 ± 0.89 | 2.31 ± 0.88 | 2.28 ± 0.87 |
RW | 2.05 ± 1.06 | 2.25 ± 1.08 | 2.17 ± 1.05 | 2.10 ± 1.10 | 2.07 ± 1.06 | 1.99 ± 1.04 | 2.05 ± 1.07 | 1.84 ± 0.97 | 1.78 ± 0.91 | 1.78 ± 0.92 | 1.76 ± 0.89 | 1.66 ± 0.87 | 1.71 ± 0.92 | 1.60 ± 0.86 | 1.64 ± 0.87 | 1.56 ± 0.86 | 1.63 ± 0.85 | 1.54 ± 0.79 | 1.51 ± 0.76 | 1.37 ± 0.65 |
PE | 1.83 ± 0.82 | 1.85 ± 0.85 | 1.83 ± 0.82 | 1.96 ± 0.88 | 1.99 ± 0.87 | 1.92 ± 0.90 | 2.00 ± 0.93 | 1.99 ± 0.96 | 1.95 ± 0.95 | 1.95 ± 0.91 | 1.97 ± 0.95 | 1.93 ± 0.94 | 1.94 ± 0.96 | 1.90 ± 0.97 | 1.87 ± 0.93 | 1.76 ± 0.88 | 1.75 ± 0.85 | 1.75 ± 0.88 | 1.74 ± 0.86 | 1.60 ± 0.81 |
MN | 4.01 ± 1.00 | 3.89 ± 0.97 | 3.92 ± 0.97 | 3.86 ± 1.01 | 3.78 ± 1.02 | 3.66 ± 0.98 | 3.67 ± 1.01 | 3.63 ± 1.02 | 3.72 ± 1.03 | 3.56 ± 1.06 | 3.61 ± 1.06 | 3.48 ± 1.05 | 3.59 ± 1.03 | 3.43 ± 1.04 | 3.52 ± 1.02 | 3.37 ± 1.10 | 3.44 ± 1.08 | 3.35 ± 1.05 | 3.43 ± 1.02 | 3.34 ± 1.01 |
Note: Except for sex, zBMI %, and SES %, depicting percent, all values represent means ± standard deviations for baseline assessment and annual follow-up within each age group. zBMI—standardized body mass index; SES—socio-economic status; CFQ—Child Feeding Questionnaire rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 to 5), with higher scores indicating higher controlling feeding practices and food-rewarding; RST—restrictive feeding; RW—food as reward; PE—pressure to eat; MN—monitoring.
3.2. Stability of Parental Feeding Practices and Standardized Child Body Mass Index
Table 2 shows the Pearson product-moment and Spearman rank order correlations between parental feeding practices and child zBMI assessed between 2 and 11 years and their mean 1-year follow-up. All variables were significantly correlated over time indicating moderate to high stability of parental feeding practices (r = 0.365–0.695, all ps < 0.01) and very high stability of child zBMI from baseline to follow-up (r = 0.818–0.951, all ps < 0.01).
Table 2.
Years of Age | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | 2 to 3 | 3 to 4 | 4 to 5 | 5 to 6 | 6 to 7 | 7 to 8 | 8 to 9 | 9 to 10 | 10 to 11 | 11 to 12 |
CFQ | ||||||||||
RST | 0.431 | 0.520 | 0.593 | 0.613 | 0.638 | 0.629 | 0.655 | 0.684 | 0.672 | 0.695 |
RW | 0.416 | 0.476 | 0.552 | 0.429 | 0.445 | 0.557 | 0.482 | 0.424 | 0.403 | 0.365 |
PE | 0.391 | 0.563 | 0.490 | 0.637 | 0.570 | 0.656 | 0.683 | 0.559 | 0.569 | 0.592 |
MN | 0.458 | 0.491 | 0.481 | 0.417 | 0.448 | 0.576 | 0.473 | 0.483 | 0.584 | 0.542 |
zBMI | 0.818 | 0.821 | 0.914 | 0.890 | 0.916 | 0.912 | 0.934 | 0.948 | 0.940 | 0.951 |
Note: All values represent r-correlation coefficients. All correlations shown were significant at a level of p < 0.01. CFQ—Child Feeding Questionnaire; RST—restrictive feeding; RW—food as reward; PE—pressure to eat; MN—monitoring; zBMI—standardized body mass index.
3.3. Continuity of Parental Feeding Practices and Standardized Child Body Mass Index
Table 3 shows the results of the paired t tests used to examine continuity of parental feeding practices and child zBMI.
Table 3.
Years of Age | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | 2 to 3 | 3 to 4 | 4 to 5 | 5 to 6 | 6 to 7 | 7 to 8 | 8 to 9 | 9 to 10 | 10 to 11 | 11 to 12 |
CFQ | ||||||||||
RST | 0.68 (327) |
2.12 (365) * |
0.85 (298) |
1.03 (357) |
0.53 (306) |
1.76 (400) |
−0.56 (371) |
1.24 (415) |
0.28 (359) |
−0.15 (362) |
RW | −3.13 (327) ** |
1.32 (365) |
1.46 (298) |
3.71 (357) *** |
0.15 (306) |
2.34 (400) * |
2.32 (371) * |
1.76 (415) |
1.90 (359) |
3.27 (362) ** |
PE | −0.34 (327) |
−3.18 (365) ** |
1.34 (298) |
0.09 (357) |
−0.15 (306) |
1.11 (400) |
1.25 (371) |
2.84 (415) ** |
0.00 (359) |
3.72 (362) *** |
MN | 2.14 (327) * |
1.12 (365) |
2.10 (298) * |
0.70 (357) |
2.58 (306) ** |
2.83 (400) ** |
2.78 (371) ** |
2.87 (415) ** |
1.78 (359) |
1.74 (362) |
zBMI | 0.33 (327) |
7.32 (365) *** |
5.61 (298) *** |
1.43 (357) |
0.74 (306) |
2.29 (400) * |
0.07 (371) |
−2.66 (415) ** |
−0.95 (359) |
−2.44 (362) * |
Note: All values represent t(df). CFQ—Child Feeding Questionnaire; RST—restrictive feeding; RW—food as reward; PE—pressure to eat; MN—monitoring; zBMI—standardized body mass index. *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
3.3.1. Restrictive Feeding
Parental restrictive feeding significantly decreased between the ages of 3 to 4 (Cohen’s d = −0.124), but showed continuity otherwise.
3.3.2. Food as Reward
Using food as reward statistically changed between the ages of 2 to 3, 5 to 6, 7 to 8, 8 to 9, and 11 to 12 (Cohen’s d = −0.183–0.175). While using food as reward rose between the ages of 2 to 3, it declined to all other follow-ups.
3.3.3. Pressure to Eat
Parental pressure to eat statistically changed between the ages of 3 to 4, 9 to 10, and 11 to 12 (Cohen’s d = −0.180–0.170). While pressure to eat increased between the ages of 3 to 4, a decrease was observed for all other follow-ups in older age ranges.
3.3.4. Monitoring
Parental monitoring statistically differed between the ages of 2 to 3, 4 to 5, 6 to 7, 7 to 8, 8 to 9, and 9 to 10 (Cohen’s d = −0.115–−0.151), consistently decreasing from baseline to follow-up.
3.3.5. Standardized Child Body Mass Index
Child zBMI significantly changed between the ages of 3 to 4, 4 to 5, 7 to 8, 9 to 10, and 11 to 12 (Cohen’s d = −0.384–0.122). While zBMI declined in younger age ranges up to 8 years, it rose in older age ranges from 9 years up.
3.4. Bi-Directional Associations between Parental Feeding Practices and Standardized Child Body Mass Index
All cross-lagged panel designs used to analyze the bi-directional associations between each parental feeding practice and child zBMI presented a RMSEA close to 0.05 or ≤ 0.05, CFI ≥ 0.95, TLI close to 0.95 or ≥ 0.95, and CMIN/df close to 2 or < 2, supporting goodness of fit (Table 4).
Table 4.
n | χ2(df) | RMSEA (p Close) | RMSEA 95% CI | CFI | TLI | CMIN/df | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Years of Age | Lower | Upper | ||||||
2 to 3 | 328 | 24.272 (23) | 0.013 (0.962) | 0.000 | 0.048 | 0.998 | 0.996 | 1.055 |
3 to 4 | 366 | 48.002 (23) ** | 0.055 (0.337) | 0.033 | 0.076 | 0.978 | 0.936 | 2.087 |
4 to 5 | 299 | 25.104 (23) | 0.018 (0.936) | 0.000 | 0.052 | 0.998 | 0.995 | 1.091 |
5 to 6 | 358 | 41.822 (23) * | 0.048 (0.529) | 0.023 | 0.071 | 0.987 | 0.963 | 1.818 |
6 to 7 | 307 | 44.900 (23) ** | 0.056 (0.321) | 0.031 | 0.080 | 0.984 | 0.953 | 1.952 |
7 to 8 | 401 | 40.227 (23) * | 0.043 (0.668) | 0.019 | 0.065 | 0.992 | 0.976 | 1.749 |
8 to 9 | 372 | 24.075 (23) | 0.011 (0.978) | 0.000 | 0.045 | 0.999 | 0.999 | 1.047 |
9 to 10 | 416 | 44.411 (23) ** | 0.047 (0.553) | 0.026 | 0.068 | 0.990 | 0.972 | 1.931 |
10 to 11 | 360 | 37.133 (23) * | 0.041 (0.699) | 0.013 | 0.065 | 0.993 | 0.979 | 1.614 |
11 to 12 | 363 | 43.443 (23) ** | 0.050 (0.482) | 0.026 | 0.072 | 0.990 | 0.972 | 1.889 |
Note: n—number of analyzed parent-child dyads. χ2(df)—chi-square test for goodness of fit (degrees of freedom); RMSEA—root-mean-square error of approximation; CI—confidence interval; CFI—comparative fit index; TLI—Tucker–Lewis index; CMIN/df—minimum discrepancy divided by its degrees of freedom. ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05. coefficients between error terms; single-headed arrows display standardized regression coefficients; dotted arrows indicate non-significant associations. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
3.4.1. Restrictive Feeding
Figure 2 shows the bi-directional associations between restrictive feeding and child zBMI. Except between the ages of 4 and 5, child zBMI consistently positively predicted restrictive feeding at follow-up (standardized β = 0.102–0.199, all ps < 0.05). Restrictive feeding at baseline did not significantly predict child zBMI at follow-up at any time (all ps ≥ 0.05).
3.4.2. Food as Reward
Figure 3 shows the bi-directional associations between using food as reward and child zBMI. Food as reward at 4 years predicted child zBMI at 5 years only (standardized β = 0.058, p = 0.025). Child zBMI at baseline did not significantly predict food as reward at follow-up at any time (all ps ≥ 0.05).
3.4.3. Pressure to Eat
Figure 4 displays the bi-directional associations between pressure to eat and child zBMI. Except between the ages of 3 and 4, child zBMI at baseline consistently negatively predicted pressure to eat at follow-up (standardized β = −0.114–−0.234, all ps < 0.01). Pressure to eat at baseline did not significantly predict child zBMI at follow-up at any time (all ps ≥ 0.05).
3.4.4. Monitoring
Figure 5 depicts the bi-directional associations between monitoring and child zBMI. Child zBMI from the age of 6 onwards consistently positively predicted monitoring at follow-up (standardized β = 0.126–0.241, all ps < 0.05). Additionally, monitoring at 5 years significantly positively predicted child zBMI at 6 years (standardized β = 0.050, p = 0.033) and monitoring at 10 years significantly negatively predicted child zBMI at 11 years (standardized β = −0.037, p = 0.035).
4. Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first prospective longitudinal study that examined the stability and continuity of parental feeding practices and standardized child BMI and their bi-directional associations in a large population-based sample across early and middle childhood, i.e., between the ages of 2 and 11 years, with a 1-year follow-up on average. As the main result, cross-lagged panel analysis showed that standardized child BMI predicted restrictive feeding, pressure to eat, and monitoring, supporting previous evidence of a child-effect model from studies with smaller age ranges [16,21,23,24,25]. There were no or only minor prospective associations found between food as reward and child zBMI. Consistently, parental feeding practices and child zBMI showed moderate to high stability across childhood. Continuity was high for restrictive feeding, and otherwise showed only minor temporal changes for the remaining feeding practices and standardized child BMI.
The result that parents used restrictive feeding practices in response to their child’s zBMI is consistent with a study in pre-school children [24]. Otherwise, the result contrasts two longitudinal studies that did not find an interaction between restrictive feeding and child zBMI in children between the ages of 2 to 5 and 7 to 11 which might be due to the relatively small sample sizes in these studies [16,30]. Inconsistency of results on the child zBMI–restriction relation might be explained by the interaction of restrictive feeding with other child characteristics (e.g., eating and activity style) as indicated in previous research showing higher restriction in 5- to 7-year-old children relatively high in hunger compared to their peers [15]. Future research is needed to examine if child zBMI solely or only in addition with other factors (e.g., child’s eating style or temperament) predicts parental restriction. Similar to previous studies [15,25,31], parental restriction did not predict child zBMI. Possibly, if parents restrict food at home, their children may seek out or binge eat food elsewhere (e.g., childcare, school, stores, social environment), counteracting the intended effect of parental restrictive feeding [13]. The lacking effect of parental restriction on child zBMI also suggests that other factors (e.g., activity style) rather than feeding practices may have a key influence on BMI development in children [1,10], highlighting the need for future research to provide enhanced understanding of risk factors influencing child BMI.
While higher standardized child BMI predicted more restrictive feeding, lower child zBMI predicted higher parental pressure to eat. This is in line with the hypothesis and previous prospective research showing strong evidence that pressure to eat is a reaction to lower child zBMI in 4- to 11-year-old children [16,23,24]. Similar to restrictive feeding, pressure to eat showed no prospective effect on child zBMI, contradicting previous longitudinal research with relatively small sample sizes in 1- to 2- and 5- to 7-year-old children [19,20]. Inconsistence in the results of the pressure to eat–child zBMI relation might be due to the different age ranges (i.e., 1- to 2- vs. 2- to 12-year-old children) or length of follow-up periods (i.e., 1 vs. 2 years). Pressure to eat possibly helps to prevent further decrease of child weight, however, the observational study design does not allow to know how the zBMI would have developed if the parents did not use pressure to eat. Experimental study designs are necessary to investigate the long-term influence of pressure to eat on child zBMI development.
The present study showed that higher standardized child BMI predicted higher levels of parental monitoring one year later in school-aged children between 6 and 11 years. In the literature, inconsistent or no effects were reported for the associations between monitoring and child zBMI [15,31], except in the study by Webber et al. [16] reporting that a higher child zBMI predicted higher monitoring in children aged 7 to 11 years. The child zBMI–monitoring relation found in this study is in line with research on covert control, a feeding practice moderately correlated with monitoring [45] which was cross-sectionally found to be associated with higher child weight status in n = 297 4- to 11-year-old British children [46]. Future research is needed to identify mediating factors on the child zBMI–monitoring relation. Notably, monitoring was found to predict child zBMI, but with weaker effects than child zBMI predicted monitoring. Specifically, monitoring predicted higher child zBMI between the ages of 5 and 6. This result is similar to previous cross-sectional research in n = 93 3- to 14-year-old children, which found a combined factor of monitoring and food-rewarding to be associated with higher child zBMI [14]. Additionally, monitoring predicted lower child zBMI in our study, but between the ages of 10 and 11 only. This could mean that the adaptation of parental feeding practices to a higher child zBMI resulted in a lower child zBMI one year later, which is in line with previous research in n = 174 Mexican American children aged 8 to 10 showing that more positive parental involvement in child food intake was associated with lower child zBMI [47]. Similarly, Jansen et al. [24] found a significant negative effect of monitoring at age 4 on child zBMI at age 6. However, when controlling for child age, child ethnicity, maternal education, and maternal BMI this effect was no longer significant. Future research is needed to clarify inconsistency of results on the monitoring–child zBMI association [11].
To our knowledge, this is the first study examining the bi-directional relationship between using food as reward and child zBMI. As a novel result, food as reward was found to predict child zBMI only between the ages of 4 and 5 years. Similarly, a study in 2-year-old children showed higher use of food as reward to predict higher weight status in children one year later [18]. The predictive effect of using food as reward on weight status in younger children only may reflect that this feeding practice is commonly used as a primary reinforcer in younger rather than older children to shape the child’s behavior [48]. Additionally, this age range is typical for “picky” eating behavior in children [49], potentially leading parents to use food as reward for eating non-preferred foods [50], which in turn might influence BMI development through an increased caloric intake. Typically, parents use palatable foods (e.g., sweets) as rewards, which are usually energy-dense and high in calories, to have a high rewarding value [51]. As frequent use of food as reward is known to enhance children’s responsiveness to food cues, refusal to try new foods, desire to eat, emotional overeating, loss of appetite for nutrient-dense foods [50], and dental caries [52], future research on the bi-directional association between food-rewarding and child BMI is needed to promote non-food rewarding strategies and thereby healthy child BMI and eating habits.
Stability of parental feeding practices from baseline to the 1-year follow-up was moderate to high confirming previous research in 1- to 7-year-old children with 1- to 3-year follow-ups even with comparable strength of correlation coefficients [19,30,31,32]. The result that stability of parental feeding practices was lowest in the 2- to 3-year-old children might be due to adaptations in parenting to child growth [49]. In addition, there are substantial variations in children’s food intake: for example, although children’s food spectrum strongly expands during this time of age, many children start “picky eating” [49]. Similarly to parental feeding practices, the finding that relative zBMI is stable in childhood is in line with evidence from an Australian sample in 5- to 10-year-old children with a 3-year follow-up [28]. Although parental feeding practices and child zBMI showed moderate to high stability, statistically significant increases or decreases in these variables were observed in different age ranges from baseline to follow-up (continuity), however, with small effects indicating only minor changes. As previous research has shown that relative weight increases overall across childhood and that child weight status shows high stability and continuity across childhood [29], the short length of the follow-up might not be enough time to detect changes in feeding practices and child zBMI. Thus, parental feeding practices and child zBMI generally seem to show fair continuity and were only subject to minor temporal changes due to the short follow-up and large sample size rather than clinically significant differences from baseline to follow-up [53].
A major strength of the present study is the large longitudinal population-based sample assessed prospectively at a wide age range using objective measurements of height and weight and an internationally established questionnaire with good psychometric properties [12] to gather data on parental feeding practices annually at multiple visits. Another strength is the separate analysis of restrictive feeding and food as reward as distinct feeding practices according to the current state of research on parental feeding practices (e.g., [37]). Although parent-child dyads were not assessed at every follow-up visit from the age of 2 years onwards, large subsamples were available for each age category. Nevertheless, it was not possible to analyze the development of parental feeding practices and child zBMI longitudinally over more than one year in the same sample, limiting the long-term perspective of our results. Although internationally established questionnaires were used to measure parental feeding practices, self-report measurement may be biased (e.g., via socially desirable response behavior). Objective measurement, for example, through video recordings of parental feeding practices, is, however, difficult to realize in a population-based cohort study. Additionally, it is possible that the high proportion of families with medium to high SES in our sample (see Table 1) is not entirely representative of the general German population, which is in line with previous research showing that a high SES is typically observed in population-based studies [54]. Further, the proportion of children with overweight and obesity in our sample (4.0%–17.9%) was lower than in the general German population (ca. 20%; [4]), probably caused in part by comparatively high SES in this study (see Table 1; [55]), which could also have influenced the results on feeding practices and child zBMI. In addition to SES and overweight, ethnicity might have influenced the study results, given cultural differences in parental feeding practices [56]. Notably, the study did not assess other potential confounders of child zBMI development and parental feeding practices, such as parental feeding practices from child birth to 2 years of age and non-parental child feeding elsewhere than at home (e.g., childcare, school, relatives; [11]).
Clinically, controlling feeding practices were found to not have the desired effect on decreasing or increasing child weight status and are known to have adverse consequences like disordered eating behaviors (e.g., loss of control eating or weight control behavior) in children in the short- and long-term [46,57]. Consequently, it is important to examine public health interventions that teach adequate feeding practices beneficial to healthy child weight status [58,59], in order to prevent negative child health development. Due to the high stability and continuity of parental feeding practices and child zBMI with slightly more fluctuations in early childhood, educational interventions should focus especially on parents with young children to promote healthy feeding practices from birth onwards and improve child nutrition across childhood. Additionally, longitudinal studies examining parental feeding practices in a more naturalistic manner (e.g., observational studies) are necessary to prevent bias resulting from self-report questionnaires (e.g., socially desirable response behavior). Further, experimental and interventional studies are warranted to examine cause and effect of parental feeding practices and child zBMI, and interventions that are best in promoting healthy child development, respectively. These further studies on the stability, continuity, and bi-directionality of parental feeding practices and child zBMI should be conducted with long-term follow-ups in order to depict the development over time. In addition, the use of heterogeneous populations (regarding SES, ethnicity, different genders of child caregivers) are recommended to enhance external validity of study results.
5. Conclusions
The current study showed moderate to high stability and individual patterns of continuity of parental feeding practices and standardized child body mass index. While our findings suggest that parents adapt their controlling feeding practices to child zBMI across all child ages, child zBMI was only positively affected by parental use of food as reward in pre-school children. Future research is warranted to examine early feeding practices and child zBMI from birth to 2 years onwards to validate whether a child-effect model can already be seen at this age range.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the ‘LIFE Child Study’ team and all parents, and children for their participation. Thanks to Katharina Eichler for proofreading the manuscript.
Abbreviations
BMI | body mass index |
CFQ | Child Feeding Questionnaire |
LIFE | Leipzig Research Center for Civilization Diseases |
MN | monitoring |
PE | pressure to eat |
RST | restrictive feeding |
RW | food as reward |
SEM | structural equation modeling |
SES | socio-economic status |
zBMI | standardized body mass index |
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, J.E., R.S., W.K. and A.H. (Anja Hilbert); Data curation, T.P., A.H. (Andreas Hiemisch) and W.K.; Formal analysis, J.E.; Funding acquisition, W.K. and A.H.(Anja Hilbert); Investigation, T.P., A.H. (Andreas Hiemisch) and W.K.; Methodology, J.E., R.S., A.H. (Andreas Hiemisch), W.K. and A.H. (Anja Hilbert); Project administration, W.K.; Resources, W.K. and A.H. (Anja Hilbert); Software, T.P.; Supervision, R.S. and A.H. (Andreas Hiemisch); Visualization, J.E.; Writing—original draft, J.E.; Writing—review and editing, J.E., R.S., T.P., A.H. (Andreas Hiemisch), W.K. and A.H. (Anja Hilbert).
Funding
Current research is supported by ‘Leipzig Research Centre for Civilization Diseases’ and the Integrated Research and Treatment Center Adiposity Diseases of Leipzig University. LIFE was funded by means of the European Union, European Regional Development Fund, and the Free State of Saxony. The German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (grant 01EO1501) funded A.H. (Anja Hilbert) and R.S. We acknowledge support from the German Research Foundation (DFG) and Leipzig University within the program of Open Access Publishing. Funding bodies played no role in the design of the study, data collection, analysis, interpretation of data, or writing of the manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interests.
References
- 1.Williams E.P., Mesidor M., Winters K., Dubbert P.M., Wyatt S.B. Overweight and obesity: Prevalence, consequences, and causes of a growing public health problem. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2015;4:363–370. doi: 10.1007/s13679-015-0169-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Karnik S., Kanekar A. Childhood obesity: A global public health crisis. Int. J. Prev. Med. 2012;3:1–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ebbeling C.B., Pawlak D.B., Ludwig D.S. Childhood obesity: Public-health crisis, common sense cure. Lancet. 2002;360:473–482. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)09678-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ng M., Fleming T., Robinson M., Thomson B., Graetz N., Margono C., Mullany E.C., Biryukov S., Abbafati C., Abera S.F., et al. Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet. 2014;384:766–781. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60460-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pulgarón E.R. Childhood obesity: A review of increased risk for physical and psychological comorbidities. Clin. Ther. 2013;35:A18–A32. doi: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2012.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Twig G., Reichman B., Afek A., Derazne E., Hamiel U., Furer A., Gershovitz L., Bader T., Cukierman-Yaffe T., Kark J.D., et al. Severe obesity and cardio-metabolic comorbidities: A nationwide study of 2.8 million adolescents. Int. J. Obes. 2018;43:1391–1399. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0213-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sutaria S., Devakumar D., Yasuda S.S., Das S., Saxena S. Is obesity associated with depression in children? Systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch. Dis. Child. 2019;104:64–74. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2017-314608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hill J.O., Wyatt H.R., Peters J.C. Energy balance and obesity. Circulation. 2012;126:126–132. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.111.087213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Weihrauch-Blüher S., Wiegand S. Risk factors and implications of childhood obesity. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2018;7:254–259. doi: 10.1007/s13679-018-0320-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Birch L.L. Child feeding practices and the etiology of obesity. Obesity. 2006;14:343–344. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Shloim N., Edelson L.R., Martin N., Hetherington M.M. Parenting styles, feeding styles, feeding practices, and weight status in 4-12 year-old children: A systematic review of the literature. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:1849. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Birch L.L., Fisher J.O., Grimm-Thomas K., Markey C.N., Sawyer R., Johnson S.L. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding Questionnaire: A measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite. 2001;36:201–210. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Savage J.S., Fisher J.O., Birch L.L. Parental influence on eating behavior: Conception to adolescence. J. Law Med. Ethics. 2007;35:22–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-720X.2007.00111.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Marshall S., Golley R., Hendrie G. Expanding the understanding of how parenting influences the dietary intake and weight status of children: A cross-sectional study. Nutr. Diet. 2011;68:127–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-0080.2011.01512.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gubbels J.S., Kremers S.P.J., Stafleu A., de Vries S.I., Goldbohm R.A., Dagnelie P.C., de Vries N.K., van Buuren S., Thijs C. Association between parenting practices and children’s dietary intake, activity behavior and development of body mass index: The KOALA Birth Cohort Study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2011;8:18. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-8-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Webber L., Cooke L., Hill C., Wardle J. Child adiposity and maternal feeding practices: A longitudinal analysis. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2010;92:1423–1428. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2010.30112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Campbell K., Andrianopoulos N., Hesketh K., Ball K., Crawford D., Brennan L., Corsini N., Timperio A. Parental use of restrictive feeding practices and child BMI z-score. A 3-year prospective cohort study. Appetite. 2010;55:84–88. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rodgers R.F., Paxton S.J., Massey R., Campbell K.J., Wertheim E.H., Skouteris H., Gibbons K. Maternal feeding practices predict weight gain and obesogenic eating behaviors in young children: A prospective study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2013;10:24. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Faith M.S., Berkowitz R.I., Stallings V.A., Kerns J., Storey M., Stunkard A.J. Parental feeding attitudes and styles and child body mass index: Prospective analysis of a gene-environment interaction. Pediatrics. 2004;114:e429–e436. doi: 10.1542/peds.2003-1075-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Farrow C.V., Blissett J. Controlling feeding practices: Cause or consequence of early child weight? Pediatrics. 2008;121:e164–e169. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Liszewska N., Scholz U., Radtke T., Horodyska K., Luszczynska A. Bi-directional associations between parental feeding practices and children’s body mass in parent-child dyads. Appetite. 2018;129:192–197. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rhee K.E., Coleman S.M., Appugliese D.P., Kaciroti N.A., Corwyn R.F., Davidson N.S., Bradley R.H., Lumeng J.C. Maternal feeding practices become more controlling after and not before excessive rates of weight gain. Obesity. 2009;17:1724–1729. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Afonso L., Lopes C., Severo M., Santos S., Real H., Durão C., Moreira P., Oliveira A. Bidirectional association between parental child-feeding practices and body mass index at 4 and 7 y of age. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2016;103:861–867. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.120824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jansen P.W., Tharner A., Van Der Ende J., Wake M., Raat H., Hofman A., Verhulst F.C., Van Ijzendoorn M.H., Jaddoe V.W., Tiemeier H. Feeding practices and child weight: Is the association bidirectional in preschool children? Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014;100:1329–1336. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.088922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Derks I.P., Tiemeier H., Sijbrands E.J., Nicholson J.M., Voortman T., Verhulst F.C., Jaddoe V.W., Jansen P.W. Testing the direction of effects between child body composition and restrictive feeding practices: Results from a population-based cohort. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2017;106:783–790. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.117.156448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Deshmukh-Taskar P., Nicklas T.A., Morales M., Yang S.J., Zakeri I., Berenson G.S. Tracking of overweight status from childhood to young adulthood: The Bogalusa Heart Study. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2006;60:48–57. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Herman K.M., Craig C.L., Gauvin L., Katzmarzyk P.T. Tracking of obesity and physical activity from childhood to adulthood: The Physical Activity Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Pediatr. Obes. 2009;4:281–288. doi: 10.3109/17477160802596171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hesketh K., Wake M., Waters E., Carlin J., Crawford D. Stability of body mass index in Australian children: A prospective cohort study across the middle childhood years. Public Health Nutr. 2004;7:303–309. doi: 10.1079/PHN2003537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Geserick M., Vogel M., Gausche R., Lipek T., Spielau U., Keller E., Pfäffle R., Kiess W., Körner A. Acceleration of BMI in early childhood and risk of sustained obesity. N. Engl. J. Med. 2018;379:1303–1312. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1803527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Farrow C., Blissett J. Stability and continuity of parentally reported child eating behaviours and feeding practices from 2 to 5 years of age. Appetite. 2012;58:151–156. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Gregory J.E., Paxton S.J., Brozovic A.M. Maternal feeding practices, child eating behaviour and body mass index in preschool-aged children: A prospective analysis. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2010;7:55. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Blissett J., Farrow C. Predictors of maternal control of feeding at 1 and 2 years of age. Int. J. Obes. 2007;31:1520–1526. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Powell F., Farrow C., Meyer C., Haycraft E. The stability and continuity of maternally reported and observed child eating behaviours and feeding practices across early childhood. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2018;15:1017. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15051017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.World Medical Association World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA. 2013;310:2191–2194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Poulain T., The LIFE Child Study Team. Baber R., Vogel M., Pietzner D., Kirsten T., Jurkutat A., Hiemisch A., Hilbert A., Kratzsch J., et al. The LIFE Child study: A population-based perinatal and pediatric cohort in Germany. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2017;32:145–158. doi: 10.1007/s10654-016-0216-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Quante M., Hesse M., Döhnert M., Fuchs M., Hirsch C., Sergeyev E., Casprzig N., Geserick M., Naumann S., Koch C., et al. The LIFE child study: A life course approach to disease and health. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:1021. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-1021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Schmidt R., Richter R., Brauhardt A., Hiemisch A., Kiess W., Hilbert A. Parental feeding practices in families with children aged 2-13 years: Psychometric properties and child age-specific norms of the German version of the Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ) Appetite. 2017;109:154–164. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.11.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kong A., Vijayasiri G., Fitzgibbon M.L., Schiffer L.A., Campbell R.T. Confirmatory factor analysis and measurement invariance of the Child Feeding Questionnaire in low-income Hispanic and African-American mothers with preschool-age children. Appetite. 2015;90:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.02.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Liu W.-H., Mallan K.M., Mihrshahi S., Daniels L.A. Feeding beliefs and practices of Chinese immigrant mothers. Validation of a modified version of the child feeding questionnaire. Appetite. 2014;80:55–60. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kromeyer-Hauschild K., Wabitsch M., Kunze D., Geller F., Geiß H.C., Hesse V., Von Hippel A., Jaeger U., Johnsen D., Korte W., et al. Perzentile für den Body-mass-Index für das Kindes- und Jugendalter unter Heranziehung verschiedener deutscher Stichproben. Mon. Kinderheilkd. 2001;149:807–818. doi: 10.1007/s001120170107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Winkler J., Stolzenberg H. Der Sozialschichtindex im Bundes-Gesundheitssurvey. Gesundheitswesen. 1999;61:178–183. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lampert T., Kroll L.E. Soziale Unterschiede in der Mortalität und Lebenserwartung. GBE Kompakt 5 (2) [(accessed on 19 June 2019)];2014 Available online: www.rki.de/gbe-kompakt.
- 43.Faul F., Erdfelder E., Buchner A., Lang A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods. 2009;41:1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Iacobucci D. Structural equations modeling: Fit indices, sample size, and advanced topics. J. Consum. Psychol. 2010;20:90–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2009.09.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Nowicka P., Flodmark C.-E., Hales D., Faith M.S. Assessment of parental overt and covert control of child’s food intake: A population-based validation study with mothers of preschoolers. Eat. Behav. 2014;15:673–678. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ogden J., Reynolds R., Smith A. Expanding the concept of parental control: A role for overt and covert control in children’s snacking behaviour? Appetite. 2006;47:100–106. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.03.330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tschann J.M., Gregorich S.E., Penilla C., Pasch L.A., De Groat C.L., Flores E., Deardorff J., Greenspan L.C., Butte N.F. Parental feeding practices in Mexican American families: Initial test of an expanded measure. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2013;10:6. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Berridge K.C. Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1996;20:1–25. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00033-B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kliegman R., Nelson W.E. Nelson Textbook of Pediatrics. 18th ed. Saunders; Philadelphia, PA, USA: 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Roberts L., Marx J.M., Musher-Eizenman D.R. Using food as a reward: An examination of parental reward practices. Appetite. 2018;120:318–326. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Drewnowski A. Energy density, palatability, and satiety: Implications for weight control. Nutr. Rev. 1998;56:347–353. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.1998.tb01677.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Colak H., Dülgergil C.T., Dalli M., Hamidi M.M. Early childhood caries update: A review of causes, diagnoses, and treatments. J. Nat. Sci. Biol. Med. 2013;4:29–38. doi: 10.4103/0976-9668.107257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ferguson C.J. An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 2009;40:532–538. doi: 10.1037/a0015808. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Jaddoe V.W.V., Van Duijn C.M., Van Der Heijden A.J., MacKenbach J.P., Moll H.A., Steegers E.A.P., Tiemeier H., Uitterlinden A.G., Verhulst F.C., Hofman A. The Generation R Study: Design and cohort update 2010. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2010;25:823–841. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9516-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Cardel M., Willig A.L., Dulin-Keita A., Casazza K., Beasley T.M., Fernández J.R. Parental feeding practices and socioeconomic status are associated with child adiposity in a multi-ethnic sample of children. Appetite. 2012;58:347–353. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Kumanyika S.K. Environmental influences on childhood obesity: Ethnic and cultural influences in context. Physiol. Behav. 2008;94:61–70. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Loth K.A., MacLehose R.F., Fulkerson J.A., Crow S., Neumark-Sztainer D. Are food restriction and pressure-to-eat parenting practices associated with adolescent disordered eating behaviors? Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2014;47:310–314. doi: 10.1002/eat.22189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Zolotarjova J., Velde G.T., Vreugdenhil A.C.E. Effects of multidisciplinary interventions on weight loss and health outcomes in children and adolescents with morbid obesity. Obes. Rev. 2018;19:931–946. doi: 10.1111/obr.12680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Bleich S.N., Vercammen K.A., Zatz L.Y., Frelier J.M., Ebbeling C.B., Peeters A. Interventions to prevent global childhood overweight and obesity: A systematic review. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2018;6:332–346. doi: 10.1016/S2213-8587(17)30358-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]