Skip to main content
PLOS Computational Biology logoLink to PLOS Computational Biology
. 2021 Feb 18;17(2):e1008737. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008737

Cholinergic neuromodulation of inhibitory interneurons facilitates functional integration in whole-brain models

Carlos Coronel-Oliveros 1,2, Rodrigo Cofré 3, Patricio Orio 1,4,*
Editor: Daniele Marinazzo5
PMCID: PMC7924765  PMID: 33600402

Abstract

Segregation and integration are two fundamental principles of brain structural and functional organization. Neuroimaging studies have shown that the brain transits between different functionally segregated and integrated states, and neuromodulatory systems have been proposed as key to facilitate these transitions. Although whole-brain computational models have reproduced this neuromodulatory effect, the role of local inhibitory circuits and their cholinergic modulation has not been studied. In this article, we consider a Jansen & Rit whole-brain model in a network interconnected using a human connectome, and study the influence of the cholinergic and noradrenergic neuromodulatory systems on the segregation/integration balance. In our model, we introduce a local inhibitory feedback as a plausible biophysical mechanism that enables the integration of whole-brain activity, and that interacts with the other neuromodulatory influences to facilitate the transition between different functional segregation/integration regimes in the brain.

Author summary

Segregation of brain activity refers to the fact that some brain regions are specialized to handle particular features of external and internal stimuli. However, to produce a coherent behavioral outcome, the brain must coordinate the activity of these specialized brain areas, and this is called integration of brain activity. Based on a fixed connectome (the brain anatomical structure), the neuromodulatory systems are one of the plausible candidates to manage the transitions of brain states in short timescales. Understanding the role of neuromodulators in brain dynamics and the segregation/integration balance is relevant, in particular, as it is known that in several neuropsychiatric disorders the segregation/integration balance its impaired. Here, we used a computational model of the whole brain to study the dual effect of the cholinergic and noradrenergic neuromodulatory systems in the switching from segregated to integrated brain states. The novelty of our work is the inclusion of a homeostatic local inhibitory loop. This specific inhibition, modulated by the cholinergic system, maintains the excitation/inhibition balance while promoting integration. Our work links the local effects of cholinergic neuromodulation, with the more global influences of the structural connectivity and neuromodulatory systems. This constitutes a step forward in the understanding of the neural mechanisms behind the segregation/integration balance of brain activity.

Introduction

Integration and segregation of brain activity are nowadays two well-established brain organization principles [14]. Functional segregation refers to the existence of specialized brain regions, allowing the local processing of information. Integration coordinates these local activities in order to produce a coherent response to complex tasks or environmental contexts [1, 2]. Both segregation and integration are required for the coherent global functioning of the brain; the balance between them constitutes a key element for cognitive flexibility, as highlighted by the theory of coordination dynamics [5, 6].

From a structural point of view, the complex functional organization of the brain is possible thanks to an anatomical connectivity that combines both integrated and segregated network characteristics, having small-world and modular properties [7]. In spite of this structural connectivity (SC) remaining fixed over short timescales, different patterns of functional connectivity (FC) can be observed during the execution of particular behavioral tasks [2]. Moreover, functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) neuroimaging studies show that during a resting state the FC is not static, but rather evolves over the recording time [810], highlighting the non-linear and non-stationary properties of the FC [11]. In a similar way, the integration and segregation of brain activity are not static over time [3, 12]. In this context, an interesting question emerges: How does the brain manage to produce dynamical transitions between different functional states from a rigid anatomical structure?.

Neuromodulatory systems tune the firing properties of neurons, providing a mechanism to change the flow of information within the brain, and allowing the transitions between different FC patterns. A recent hypothesis proposed by Shine [13] argues that neuromodulation allows the transition between integrated and segregated states, manipulating the neural gain function [14]. In that line, the cholinergic and noradrenergic systems have been proposed as candidates to influence the cognitive processing within the brain [15, 16], in spite of not being the unique neuromodulatory systems in the central nervous system which can tune the firing properties of neurons [14, 17].

The cholinergic system is involved in cognitive and attentional selectivity [16], and in the cerebral cortex the main source of acetylcholine are projections from the basal forebrain [18]. Acetylcholine increments the overall excitability [19, 20], and consequently rises population activity above noise, a mechanism referred as response gain [14]. The increase in signal-to-noise ratio, especially in brain areas that are close to each other, promotes segregation when considering the response gain by itself [13]. On the other hand, the noradrenergic system is related to the exploratory behavior [15], and the principal source of noradrenergic projections to the cerebral cortex comes from the locus coeruleus [21]. Noradrenaline increases the reponsivity (or selectivity) of neuronal populations to input-driven activity (e.g., sensory stimuli, inputs for distant brain areas relevant to a task) with respect to spontaneous activity (or the internal state of the brain) [2224], filtering out noise [25] in a mechanism called filter gain [14]. The effect of the noradrenergic system in increasing the signal-to-noise ratio facilitates the detection of signals embedded in a noisy environment [25], boosting the signal detection and promoting integration [13]. Therefore, a complex interaction between the cholinergic and noradrenergic system seems to manage the balance between integration and segregation.

Using a whole-brain model, Shine et al. [26] showed that neuromodulation and integration follow an inverted-U relationship. If one considers that neuromodulation also shows an inverted-U relationship with in-task performance, [15, 27], it is possible to hypothesize that neuromodulatory systems boost cognitive and attentional performance by increasing the functional integration in the brain, as proposed by Shine et al‥ [13, 26].

There are still unanswered questions about the specific effects of neuromodulation on integration and segregation. Experimental research points out that the cholinergic system, through both nicotinic and muscarinic receptors, boosts the signal-to-noise ratio in two principal ways [14, 28]: first, increasing the excitability of pyramidal neurons [2931], and second, enhancing the firing rates of dendritic-targeting GABAergic interneurons –an effect that promotes a focused intra-columnar inhibition, reducing the local excitatory feedback to pyramidal neurons [3133]. Consequently, pyramidal neurons become more responsive to stimulus from other distant regions respect to the stimulus of its own cortical column [28, 29, 34]. The particular effect of the cholinergic system on excitatory neurons was one of the focus of the whole-brain simulation work by Shine et al‥ [26]. However, the cholinergic modulation of inhibitory interneurons and its effect on the segregation/integration balance has not been analyzed at the whole-brain level. This is the main focus of the present work.

Here, we use an in silico approach to analyze the effect of neuromodulatory systems on functional integration in the brain, focusing on the cholinergic action in inhibitory interneurons. We combined a real human structural connectivity with the Jansen & Rit neural mass model of cortical columns [35, 36]. The mesoscopic properties of the model enable us to study more specifically the effects of neuromodulators in whole-brain dynamics. To make our simulations more comparable to experimental findings [3, 12, 37, 38], and also following Shine et al. [26], fMRI blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals were generated from the firing rates of pyramidal neurons. Integration and segregation were then assessed in the functional connectivity matrices derived from the BOLD-like signals, using a graph theoretical approach.

The neuromodulation was discerned in three components. First, we included an “excitatory gain”, which increases the inter-columnar coupling. In our model, this gain mechanism is mediated by the action of the cholinergic system on pyramidal neurons, having an indirect effect on their excitability [13, 14, 28]. Second, we added an “inhibitory gain”, also mediated by the cholinergic system, that controls the inputs from inhibitory to excitatory interneurons and reduces the local feedback excitation. This additional connection, well described in cortical columns [39, 40], represents a modification of the original neural mass model proposed by Jansen & Rit [35, 36]. Finally, we incorporated a “filter gain”, that increments the pyramidal neurons’ sigmoid function slope [14]. This gain mechanism is mediated by the noradrenergic system; it acts as a filter, decreasing (increasing) the responsivity to weak (strong) stimuli [23, 25], boosting signal-to-noise ratio. We show that the increase of the signal-to noise ratio, mediated both by the excitatory and filter gains, and the decrease in the feedback excitation, related to the inhibitory gain, promote functional integration.

Results

We assessed the effect of the neuromodulatory systems using a whole-brain neural mass model of brain activity. In the model, each node corresponds to a brain area and is represented by a neural mass consisting of three populations [35, 36]: pyramidal neurons (that reside in cortical column layer V), excitatory interneurons (nearby pyramidal cells which reside in the same layer than the principal pyramidal population), and inhibitory interneurons (Fig 1A). Based on Silberberg & Markram [39] and Fino et al. [40], we have added a connection from inhibitory interneurons from excitatory interneurons (thick line in Fig 1A), allowing us to study the effect of its modulation by cholinergic influences (see below). The nodes are connected through a weighted and undirected structural connectivity matrix derived from human data [41], parcellated in 90 cortical and sub-cortical regions with the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) atlas [42] (Fig 1B). Connections between nodes are made by pyramidal neurons, considering that long-range projections are mainly excitatory [43, 44]. Using the firing rates of each node as inputs to a generalized hemodynamic model [45], we obtained fMRI BOLD signals from which we calculated integration and segregation of the resulting FC matrices.

Fig 1. Whole-brain neural mass model.

Fig 1

A) The Jansen & Rit model is constituted by a population of pyramidal neurons with excitatory and inhibitory feedback mediated by interneurons (INs). Each population is connected by a series of constants Ci. The outputs are transformed from average pulse density to average postsynaptic membrane potential by an excitatory (inhibitory) impulse response function hE(t) (hI(t)). Then, a sigmoid function S performs the inverse operation. Pyramidal neurons project to distant cortical columns, and receive both uncorrelated Gaussian-distributed inputs p(t) and inputs from other cortical columns z(t). Neuromodulation is constituted by three parameters, colored in red: excitatory gain α, which scales z(t), inhibitory gain β, which increases the inhibitory input to excitatory INs (thick line), and filter gain, r0, which modifies the slope of the sigmoid function in pyramidal neurons. B) Each node represents a cortical column, whose dynamics is ruled by the Jansen & Rit equations. Nodes are connected through a structural connectivity matrix, M C) Neuromodulation modifies the coupling between neurons and the properties of the input (average postsynaptic membrane potential) to output (average pulse density) sigmoid function. The cholinergic system modifies the global coupling and local inhibition. α amplifies the response of pyramidal neurons to other columns’ input; it also increases pyramidal neurons excitability. β amplifies the effect of inhibitory INs to excitatory INs, damping pyramidal cells excitability. The noradrenergic system increments the responsivity of pyramidal neurons to relevant stimuli respect to noise, as a filter, by increasing the slope r0 of their sigmoid function.

Following Shine et al. [26], we modeled the influence of the cholinergic and noradrenergic systems through the manipulation of the response and filter gains, respectively (Fig 1C). The principal difference in our approach is that we split the response gain in excitatory gain (long-range pyramidal to pyramidal coupling), α, and inhibitory gain (local inhibitory to excitatory interneurons coupling), β. While the excitatory gain boosts the pyramidal neurons responsivity to long-range inputs, and indirectly increases the pyramidal cells excitability, the inhibitory gain reduces the local excitatory feedback from interneurons. Finally, the filter gain r0 modifies the sigmoid function slope of pyramidal neurons, increasing its responsivity to relevant stimuli and boosting signal-to-noise ratio. Here, we studied the combined effect of the three gain mechanisms to understand how neuromodulatory systems shape the global neuronal dynamics in two different timescales: EEG-like and BOLD-like signals. Our hypothesis is that the inhibitory gain will play a significant role in increasing the likelihood of integration.

Inhibitory gain facilitates neuronal coordination

We first studied the combined influence of the excitatory and inhibitory response gains, by fixing r0 = 0.56 mV−1 (its default value) and then simulating neuronal activity at different combinations of α ∈ [0, 1] and β ∈ [0, 0.5]. Then, we analyzed the graph properties of the static (time-averaged) functional connectivity (sFC) matrices, obtained from the pairwise Pearson’s correlations of BOLD-like signals. Namely, we calculated the global efficiency Ew, a measure of integration defined as the inverse of the characteristic path length [46], and modularity Qw, a measure of segregation based on the detection of network communities or modules [46]. High values of Ew represent an efficient coordination between all pairs of nodes in the network, a signature of integration. In contrast, a high modularity Qw is associated to segregation [46].

Fig 2A shows that functional integration (Ew) is maximized in an intermediate region of the (α, β) parameter space; and that integration is accompanied by a decrease in the segregation (Qw). Also, the system undergoes a sharp transition crossing a critical boundary. The transitions between different regimes are better appreciated in Fig 2B, where we show a 1-D sweep of α at β = 0.25. Dashed lines at α = 0.3 and α = 0.8 correspond to points in the parameter space where drastic changes in dynamic properties of the network occur. Further, the global efficiency Ew follows an inverted-U relationship with the excitatory neuromodulation, suggesting (in our whole-brain model) that optimal levels of cholinergic neuromodulation maximize functional integration (see Discussion). Also, Qw peaks higher at the right critical boundary (dashed lines). A 1-D sweep of β at α = 0.5 (Fig 2C), shows an increase in integration crossing the critical transition at β = 0.1. These results are replicated using other measures of integration and segregation: the mean participation coefficient PCw, an integration metric that quantifies the between-modules connectivity, and the transitivity Tw, which accounts for segregation counting triangular motifs [46] (S1 Fig).

Fig 2. Network features in the (α, β) parameter space.

Fig 2

A) Global efficiency Ew (integration) and modularity Qw (segregation) of the graphs derived from the sFCs of the BOLD-like signals. B) Transitions through critical boundaries in the α axis, for a fixed β = 0.25. Transition points are represented by black dashed lines at α = 0.3 and α = 0.8. C) Transitions in the β axis, for a fixed α = 0.5, with a critical transition at β = 0.1.

The modulation of the inhibitory gain (β) shows an important effect on the integration and segregation properties of the whole network measured by the global efficiency and modularity, respectively. This could be due to the reduction of excitatory feedback only, or to a more specific effect of the newly introduced connection from inhibitory to excitatory interneurons. In the first case, we expect a similar effect by reducing the C1 parameter (see Fig 1A) because this also reduces the excitatory feedback loop of the cortical columns. As shown in S2 Fig, this is only partially the case. The reduction of the C1 connection weight –in the absence of the inhibitory to excitatory interneuron connection– enables the network to reach integration but in a smaller region of the parameter space and to a lower extent than the inhibitory modulation that we introduced in our model.

To show in more detail how each gain mechanism produces integrated or segregated functional network states, we present in Fig 3 some BOLD-like signals and their respective sFC matrices. We chose five tuples of (α, β) parameters, marked with the red circles in the Fig 3A. Functional integration measured by global efficiency is maximal in the middle (α = 0.5, β = 0.25), and segregation measured by modularity is maximized far away from this point (α = 0.25, β = 0.125, and α = 0.75, β = 0.375). In the extreme cases (α = 0, β = 0, and α = 1, β = 0.5) there is neither integration nor segregation; in the first case the network is disconnected, and in the second one the system crossed the second bifurcation point and pyramidal neurons are not oscillating (neurons are over-excited).

Fig 3. fMRI-like sFCs at different values of α and β.

Fig 3

A) The red circles represent pairs of (α, β) values in which different integration/segregation profiles can be observed. B-F) BOLD-like signals, and their respective sFC matrices, for the (α, β) values shown in A. The sFC networks evolve from neither integration nor segregation (B, the nodes are disconnected), to a more integrated sFC (C). In D the integration is maximal, and a further increase of both parameters produces a more segregated sFC matrix (E). Finally, in F there is neither integration nor segregation (the pyramidal neurons are over-excited). We shown only 120 s of BOLD-like signals, while sFC matrices were built with the full-length time series (600 s).

Inhibitory gain allows the noradrenaline-mediated integration

To further validate our model, we sought to reproduce the results of the neuromodulatory paradigm proposed by Shine et al‥ [13, 26]. We characterized the relationship between neuromodulation and integration in the (α, r0) parameter space, with α ∈ [0, 1] and r0 ∈ [0, 1] while leaving β fixed at 0 or 0.4 (without and with inhibitory gain, respectively). The results for β = 0 (Fig 4A) show no integration in the entire parameter space. On the other hand, the observations of Shine et al. [26] are fully reproduced with β = 0.4 (Fig 4B). Similar results hold for the mean PCw and Tw, as shown in S3 Fig.

Fig 4. Network features in the (α, r0) parameter space.

Fig 4

A-B) Global efficiency Ew (integration) and modularity Qw (segregation) of the graphs derived from the sFCs of the BOLD-like signals, for A) β = 0 (no action of the inhibitory gain) and B) β = 0.4. C) Transitions through the critical boundary in the α axis, with a fixed r0 = 1 mV−1 and β = 0.4. Critical transition points represented by black dashed lines at α = 0.3 and α = 0.8. D) Transitions in the r0 axis, for a fixed α = 0.6 and β = 0.4, with a critical transition at r0 = 0.33 mV−1.

As observed previously in Fig 2, critical boundaries delimit asynchronous and synchronous states in the (α, r0) parameter space. A 1-D sweep of α at r0 = 1 mV−1 shows a sharp transition (Fig 4C) (Fig 4B shows that this is also true for lower values of r0). Global efficiency Ew increases alongside the decrease of modularity Qw, and further increments of α produce network desynchronization. On the other hand, a 1-D sweep of r0 at α = 0.6 (Fig 4D) produces similar observations, but just one boundary is visible. As in the (α, β) parameter space, the excitatory gain α follows an inverted-U relationship with integration. This relationship was not observed between the filter gain r0 and integration.

In the whole-brain model, the cholinergic system exerts its effect by changing both α and β parameters. Under this assumption, a logical consequence of the cholinergic neuromodulation is the possibility of α and β increasing/decreasing simultaneously. For that reason, we repeated the analysis previously performed in the (α, r0) parameter space, but this time we changed β alongside α following the relationship β = 0.5α. The results are shown in S4 Fig. They are similar to those in Fig 4, but this time the relationship between r0 and Ew is no longer a sigmoid-like function, and instead it follows an inverted-U relationship.

Changes in the EEG timescale match with the increase of integration

Previous experimental and theoretical works [13, 14, 28] suggest that neuromodulatory systems increase the signal-to-noise ratio, allowing neuronal populations to be sensitive to local or distant populations to a greater extent than noise. To test that, we measured the signal-to noise-ratio (SNR) using the power spectral density (PSD) function of each EEG-like signal (see Methods) and report the average value over all nodes. Additionally, we computed the average Kuramoto order parameter R¯ [47], as a measure of global synchronization in the fast timescale of EEG. Values of R¯ closer to 1 indicate a perfect in-phase synchronization, and values closer to 0 indicate complete asynchrony.

Both SNR and R¯ match the region of integration, measured as the global efficiency Ew in the slowest BOLD timescale (Fig 5), supporting the idea that neuromodulatory systems promote integration by increasing SNR. In consequence, our results link in two different timescales the effect of neuromodulation in the coordination of brain activity. These results are not possible without the action of the inhibitory gain (β = 0, Fig 5A).

Fig 5. EEG features in the (α, r0) parameter space.

Fig 5

A-B) Average phase synchrony R¯ and signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) measured from the EEG-like signals. A) No action of inhibitory gain (β = 0). B) The increase of R¯ and the SNR matches with functional integration for β = 0.4. C) Transitions through the critical boundary in the α axis, with a fixed r0 = 1 mV−1. Critical transitions represented by black dashed lines at α = 0.3 and α = 0.8. D) Transitions in the r0 axis, for a fixed α = 0.6, with a critical transition at r0 = 0.33 mV−1.

Dynamical richness peaks near the critical boundary

As suggested by experimental [3] and computational studies [26], a shift to more segregated or integrated functional states decreases the topological variability of the network. Also, near the critical transitions for segregation to integration, network variability and communicability peak [26]. We tested this hypothesis performing a Functional Connectivity Dynamics (FCD) analysis [9, 10] on the BOLD-like signals, using the sliding windows approach depicted in Fig 6A–6C [48]. The resulting time vs time FCD matrix captures the concurrence of FC patterns, visualized as square blocks. We computed the variance of the FCD, var(FCD), as a multistability index [48], where values greater than 0 indicate the switching between different FC patterns. Additionally, we calculated the FCD speed dtyp as described by Battaglia et al. [49], which captures how fast the FC patterns fluctuate over time. Values closer to 1 indicate a continuous change of diverse FC patterns, and closer to 0 the concurrence of stable and similar states over time.

Fig 6. Analysis of functional connectivity dynamics.

Fig 6

A) Sample fMRI BOLD time series showing the fixed length and overlapping time windows at the begginging. In color, the time windows corresponding to the FCs shown in B. B) FC matrices obtained in the colored time windows. C) Functional Connectivity Dynamics (FCD) matrix, where all the FCs obtained were vectorized and then compared against each other using a vector-based distance (Clarkson distance). D) FCD matrices through the critical boundary, in both α and r0 direction. Below each FCD, a histogram of its upper triangular values is shown. The variance of these values constitutes a measure of multistability.

In Fig 6D we show a set of FCD matrices obtained at different values of α and r0, together with histograms of their off-diagonal values. Red FCD matrices (with high values) correspond to incoherent states, as the FC continuously evolves in time. On the other hand, a blue FCD matrix (with low values) indicates a fixed FC throughout the simulation. Multistability is higher for green/yellow patchy matrices, because this indicates FC patterns that change and also repeat over time. As can be inferred observing the FCD distributions, the variance of the values in the histograms –var(FCD)– can be used as a measure of multistability [48].

Fig 7 shows how multistability (var(FCD)) and FCD speed change in the whole (α, r0) space, for β = 0.4. At low levels of both α and r0, the neuronal activity is constituted mainly by noisy asynchronous signals, conditions associated to low (near 0) values of var(FCD), and with a high dtyp (all FC patterns differ from each other, as expected for noise-driven signals) (Fig 7A). In the other extreme, for r0 > 0.5 mV−1 and α ∈ [0.5, 0.6], values that correspond to the integrated states, var(FCD) is also small and dtyp falls close to 0. In consequence, integrated states are more stable and less susceptible to network reconfiguration over time. In contrast, var(FCD) peaks near the critical boundary, through the α and r0 axes (Fig 7B and 7C). Moreover, crossing the boundaries is associated with a continuous decrease of dtyp: the emerging integration mediated by gain mechanisms is associated with more stable FC patterns over time.

Fig 7. Dynamical features of the system in the (α, r0) parameter space for β = 0.4.

Fig 7

A) Multistability var(FCD) and typical FCD speed dtyp measured from the Functional Connectivity Dynamics analysis (BOLD-like signals). B) Transitions through the critical boundary in the α axis, with a fixed r0 = 1 mV−1. Critical transitions represented by black dashed lines at α = 0.3 and α = 0.8. C) Transitions in the r0 axis, for a fixed α = 0.6, with a critical transition at r0 = 0.33 mV−1.

Discussion

In this work, we used a whole-brain neural mass model to investigate how local (meso-scale) neuromodulatory effects can impact the global functional network properties. Importantly, we studied the effect that the cholinergic system has in both, excitatory and inhibitory neurons, along with the noradrenergic modulatory influence. Our model shows an increase in functional integration at intermediate values of the parameters that resemble the cholinergic and noradrenergic systems, following an inverted-U relation with the neuromodulation. In addition, the modulation of an intra-columnar inhibitory gain can promote functional integration and facilitates the effect of the other neuromodulatory systems. Finally, we show that our results hold for both the EEG and fMRI timescales, and that integration is accompanied by an increment in the signal-to-noise ratio, as well as a reduction of dynamical variability captured by the FCD analysis.

Our main motivation was to study the large-scale effects of the cholinergic neuromodulation of local inhibitory circuits. Although the cholinergic system increases the response of pyramidal neurons to external afferences through nicotinic and muscarinic receptors [34], the same system can promote intra-columnar inhibition, an effect mediated by nicotinic receptors expressed by inhibitory interneurons [28, 31, 50]. A possible consequence is the increase of the influence of external inputs, in comparison with the local intra-columnar inputs, shifting the flow of information from local to global processing. Based on these experimental findings [28, 31, 34, 50], we hypothesized that the cholinergic neuromodulation of the inhibitory interneurons facilitates functional integration. Our model shows that the action of the cholinergic system, on both the excitation of pyramidal neurons and the intra-columnar inhibitory feedback, can shift the system towards a functionally integrated regime. In this way, we propose a plausible biophysical mechanism of inhibitory-to-excitatory interneuron connection that facilitates functional integration of brain activity (see Fig 4).

The new intra-columnar connection that we introduced in the Jansen & Rit model –represented by the inhibitory gain β–, produces a higher dampening of the excitatory input to pyramidal neurons when their excitability is high. Conversely, when the pyramidal excitability decreases, the effect of the inhibitory loop between interneurons is low, and the excitatory loop can rise the excitability of pyramidal cells. In this way, the inhibitory gain provides a simple dynamical mechanism to homeostatically preserve the excitation/inhibition (E/I) balance at the node level. In contrast, a simple reduction of the excitatory feedback (e.g., decreasing C1, S2 Fig), fails to compensate the E/I balance when pyramidal excitability is low, and thus has a limited ability to promote functional integration. This highlights the role of specific intra-columnar inhibitory feedback connections in shaping the network behavior, and justifies our modification of the model with a homeostatic mechanism. Similar types of inhibition-mediated control of the E/I balance have been implemented in a dynamic mean-field model [51] as well as in the Wilson-Cowan model [52]. Remarkably, the E/I balance has been considered a determinant element in the interplay between integration and segregation [53]

Our results also have an interpretation from the dynamical systems perspective. It has been proposed that, at rest, brain activity operates near a bifurcation point, where segregated (uncoordinated) and integrated (coordinated) regimes alternate in time. Then, a shift to more segregated or integrated states takes places with a change in behavioral context [5, 9, 10]. At the node level, the Jansen & Rit model has two Hopf supercritical bifurcations [54]. When α and r0 are low, the node dynamics is defined by a stable focus (a fixed point with non-monotonic convergence), and thus pyramidal outputs consist of low amplitude noisy signals. Increasing both parameters causes the bifurcation into an unstable focus within a limit cycle, with high amplitude oscillations. Increasing α further produces a new bifurcation (into a stable focus) and the limit cycle disappears. The inhibitory gain β constitutes a mechanism to keep the model working between the two bifurcations points, allowing the transitions between different functional states (more segregated or integrated). This again highlights the role of β as an inhibitory control loop which preserves the E/I balance and sustains a richer brain dynamics.

Several clues suggest that the model we propose is in the right track. First, our model reproduces previous results of Shine et al., also using a whole-brain model [26]: they reported and inverted-U relationship between cholinergic neuromodulation and integration, an increase in phase synchronization, and that integration is accompanied by a reduction in the time-resolved topological variability (captured, in our analysis, by the variance of the FCD). Second, functional integration matches with an increase of SNR, a common effect attributed to neuromodulatory systems [13, 14, 28, 55]. Third, integration is accompanied in our model by a reduction in oscillatory frequency –which falls within the Theta range of EEG spectrum– (S8 Fig); an effect that is also perceived in several cognitive tasks [56, 57], and reproduced by other computational studies [58, 59].

From an experimental point of view, an increase in the local and global efficiency (integration) in fMRI has been reported after the administration of nicotine both in resting state conditions [37] and during an attentional task [38]. Interestingly, the performance was positively correlated with the global efficiency, and negatively correlated with the average clustering coefficient. Some nicotinic agonists have pro-cognitive effects as well, in health and disease [60]. Considering the relationship between functional integration and cognition [2, 3, 13], our model suggests that the possible pro-cognitive effects associated with the cholinergic system are due to a selective increase in the excitability of excitatory and inhibitory neural populations within brain areas. Thus, our computational approach –in the same spirit as Wylie et al. [37] and Gießing et al. [38]– links the meso-scale consequences of inhibitory interneurons neuromodulation with the functional network topology features at the whole-brain level.

On the other hand, the inverted-U relationship between neuromodulation and integration that we are reporting in our whole-brain model, has not been experimentally observed. However, an inverted-U function between cholinergic and noradrenergic neuromodulation and in-task performance [15, 27]; as well as between in-task performance and functional integration [12] has been reported. Taking these together we propose, in accordance to Shine et al‥ [13, 26], that neuromodulation improves cognitive performance by boosting integration. The results from computational modeling should nevertheless be verified by measuring both integration and cognitive performance as functions of neuromodulatory activity, e.g., as functions of the dose of a cholinergic/noradrenergic drug.

There is a lot of room for further progress starting from this work. Future research may consider the addition of neuromodulatory maps [41, 61] in order to take into account the heterogeneous expression of the receptors, or explore models that can reproduce the effect of other neuromodulatory systems [17]. Furthermore, it is known that cholinergic and noradrenergic projections have a specific spatial organization [62, 63]. Our model considers neuromodulation to be static, that is, the parameters α, β and r0 do not change over time, as in tonic neuromodulation. An improvement to our model may be the addition of the release and reuptake dynamics of neuromodulators, as in Kringelbach et al. [64] or the characterization of the dynamics under acute neuromodulatory ‘pulses’. Other interneurons subtypes and their modulation could be included –such as fast-spiking inhibitory interneurons– to account for the faster EEG features of brain activity [65]. Finally, the graph theoretical analysis used here only considers pairwise interactions, neglecting high-order effects that may contain important information about high dimensional functional brain interactions. Information-theoretical [66, 67] and algebraic topological approaches [6870] may provide complementary insights of high-order interdependencies in the brain.

Our findings shed light on a better understanding of neurophysiological mechanisms involved in the functional integration and segregation of the human brain activity and constitutes a step forward from the neuromodulatory framework proposed by Shine [13], including the role of a second cholinergic target and also highlighting the role of a homeostatic inhibitory feedback. This line of research may have plentiful of scientific and clinical implications, as a vast body of evidence suggest that functional integration and segregation may be altered in neuropsychiatric disorders [53, 71, 72], e.g, in Alzheimer disease and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) [73, 74]. These results point out the usefulness of graph theoretical analysis to exhibit functional markers for characterizing and understanding neuropsychiatric disorders. Understanding the neuromodulatory mechanisms that underlie the imbalances of integration and segregation will lead to a more profound understanding of how the brain works in health and disease and to future progress in pharmacological treatments.

Methods

Whole-brain neural mass model

To simulate neuronal activity we used a modified version of the Jansen & Rit neural mass model [35, 36]. In this model, a cortical column consists of a population of pyramidal neurons (that reside in cortical column layer V) with projections to other two populations: excitatory interneurons (nearby pyramidal cells which reside in the same layer than the principal pyramidal population) and inhibitory interneurons; both project back to the pyramidal population. The dynamical evolution of the three populations within the cortical column is modeled by two blocks each. The first transforms the average pulse density in average postsynaptic membrane potential (which can be either excitatory or inhibitory) (Fig 1A). This block, denominated post synaptic potential (PSP) block, is represented by an impulse response function

hE(t)={Aate-at,t00,t<0 (1)

for the excitatory outputs, and

hI(t)={Bbte-bt,t00,t<0, (2)

for the inhibitory ones. The constants A and B define the maximum amplitude of the PSPs for the excitatory (EPSPs) and inhibitory (IPSPs) cases respectively, while a and b represent the inverse time constants for the excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic action potentials, respectively. The second block transforms the postsynaptic membrane potential in average pulse density, and is given by a sigmoid function of the form

S(ν,r)=ζmax1+er(θ-ν), (3)

with ζmax as the maximum firing rate of the neuronal population, r the slope of the sigmoid function, and θ the half maximal response of the population.

Additionally, the pyramidal neurons receive an external stimulus p(t), whose values were taken from a Gaussian distribution with mean μ = 2 impulses/s and standard deviation σ = 2. Different values of σ were explored; qualitatively the results are similar for different σ values, but the magnitude of integration decreases with σ. This exploration is shown in the S5 Fig. In the same manner, the mean of the Gaussian distribution μ has an effect in decreasing the synchronization and integration, as shown in the S5 Fig.

To study the effect of the neuromodulatory systems at the macro-scale level, we included long-range pyramidal-to-pyramidal neurons and short-range inhibitory-to-excitatory interneurons couplings, to mimic the effects of neuromodulation through the excitatory and inhibitory gain parameters, respectively. This short-range coupling between interneurons, well described at the meso-scale level [39, 40], constitutes a modification of the original equations. A bifurcation analysis of the model at different values of β reveals that this parameter shifts the oscillatory regime of the system towards larger values of the external input p(t) (S7 Fig). Nevertheless, the main oscillatory rhythm of the model is well maintained within the α band frequency (around 10 Hz).

In the model presented in the Fig 1A, each node i ∈ [1…N] represents a single brain area. The nodes are connected by a normalized structural connectivity matrix M˜ (Fig 1B). This matrix is derived from a human connectome [41] parcellated in n = 90 cortical and subcortical regions with the automated anatomical labelling (AAL) atlas [42]; the matrix is undirected and takes values between 0 and 1. Because long-range connections are mainly excitatory [43, 44], only links between the pyramidal neurons of a node i with pyramidal neurons of a node j are considered. We applied a local normalization procedure to the structural connectivity matrix M. The normalization consisted in dividing all the columns belonging to a node i by the in-strength of the node. The entries of the resulting normalized matrix M˜ are defined as

M˜ij=Mijj=1,jinMij (4)

The local normalization procedure constitutes a form of homeostatic plasticity, which equalizes the excitatory inputs that the nodes receive, while preserving the structural topology. It has been reported that this mechanism improves the fit of a whole-brain mesoscopic model to empirical fMRI data, and leads to a better estimation of the functional connectivity [75].

The overall set of equations, for a node i, includes the within and between nodes activity

x˙0,i(t)=y0,i(t)y˙0,i(t)=Aa[S(C2x1,i(t)-C4x2,i(t)+Cαzi(t),r0)]-2ay0,i(t)-a2x0,i(t)x˙1,i(t)=y1,i(t)y˙1,i(t)=Aa[p(t)+S(C1x0,i(t)-Cβx2,i,r1)]-2ay1,i(t)-a2x1,i(t)x˙2,i(t)=y2,i(t)y˙2,i(t)=Bb[S(C3x0,i(t),r2)]-2by2,i(t)-b2x2,i(t)x˙3,i(t)=y3,i(t)y˙3,i(t)=Aa¯[S(C2x1,i(t)-C4x2,i(t)+Cαzi(t),r0)]-2a¯y3,i(t)-a¯i2x3,i(t) (5)

where x0, x1, x2 correspond to the outputs of the PSP blocks of the pyramidal neurons, and excitatory and inhibitory interneurons, respectively, and x3 the long-range outputs of pyramidal neurons. The constants C1, C2, C3 and C4 scale the connectivity between the neural populations (see Fig 1A). The first pair of equations, x0 and y0, are related to the outputs of pyramidal cells to both interneurons; the second pair, x1 and y1, represent all the local excitatory inputs that the pyramidal neurons receive; x2 and y2 constitute the inhibitory contribution to pyramidal cells; and finally, x3 and y3 correspond to the long-range excitatory outputs of pyramidal neurons. We used the original parameter values of Jansen & Rit [35, 36]: ζmax = 5 s−1, θ = 6 mV, r0 = r1 = r2 = 0.56 mV−1, a = 100 s−1, b = 50 s−1, A = 3.25 mV, B = 22 mV, C1 = C, C2 = 0.8C, C3 = 0.25C, C4 = 0.25C, and C = 135. The parameters A, B, a and b were selected as in the original Jansen & Rit model [35, 36] to produce IPSPs longer in amplitude and latency in comparison with the EPSPs. The inverse of the characteristic time constant for the long-range EPSPs was defined as a¯=0.5a. This choice was based on the fact that long-range excitatory inputs of pyramidal neurons target their apical dendrites, and consequently this decreases the time course of the EPSPs at the soma due to dendritic nonlinearities and a gradient of input impedances [76].

The overall input from other cortical columns ji to the column i is given by

zi(t)=j=1,jinM˜ijx3,j(t) (6)

The average PSP of pyramidal neurons in column i characterizes the EEG-like signal in the source space; it is computed as [35, 36]

νi(t)=C2x1,i(t)-C4x2,i(t)+Cαzi(t) (7)

The firing rates of pyramidal neurons ζi(t) = S(νi(t), r0) were used to simulate the fMRI BOLD recordings. The parameters α, β and r0 account for the influence of the neuromodulatory systems (Fig 1C), as described in next subsection.

Neuromodulation

The effects of the cholinergic system were modeled by the parameters α and β. The parameter α increases the long-rage pyramidal to pyramidal neuron coupling through the M˜ matrix. Although α does not control directly the excitability, increasing α amplifies the input to pyramidal neurons [13, 14]. The parameter β scales the short-range inhibitory-to-excitatory interneurons coupling, decreasing the recurrent excitation to pyramidal neurons [28]. We refer to α as the excitatory gain, and β as the inhibitory gain. In comparison with the current framework proposed by Shine [13], the novelty of our neuromodulatory approach is the inclusion of the inhibitory gain to the model. The effect of the noradrenergic system, designated as filter gain, was simulated controlling the parameter r0, which represents the sigmoid function slope of the pyramidal population, and increases the signal-to-noise ratio of pyramidal cells [14, 25].

Simulation

Following Birn et al. [77], we ran simulations to generate the equivalent of 11 min real-time recordings, discarding the first 60 s. The system of differential equations (Eq (5)) was solved with the Euler–Maruyama method, using an integration step of 1 ms. We used six random seeds which controlled the initial conditions and the stochasticity of the simulations. We simulated neuronal activity sweeping the parameters α ∈ [0, 1], β ∈ [0, 0.5] and r0 ∈ [0, 1]. All the simulations were implemented in Python and the codes are freely available at https://github.com/vandal-uv/Neuromod2020.

Simulated fMRI BOLD signals

We used the firing rates ζi(t) to simulate BOLD-like signals from a generalized hemodynamic model [45]. An increment in the firing rate ζi(t) triggers a vasodilatory response si, producing blood inflow fi, changes in the blood volume vi and deoxyhemoglobin content qi. The corresponding system of differential equations is

si˙(t)=ζi(t)-si(t)τs-fi(t)-1τffi˙(t)=si(t)vi˙(t)=fi(t)-vi(t)1/κτvqi˙(t)=fi(t)(1-(1-E0)1/fi(t))E0-qi(t)vi(t)1/κvi(t)τq, (8)

where τs, τf, τv and τq represent the time constants for the signal decay, blood inflow, blood volume and deoxyhemoglobin content, respectively. The stiffness constant (resistance of the veins to blood flow) is given by κ, and the resting-state oxygen extraction rate by E0. Finally, the BOLD-like signal of node i, denoted Bi(t), is a non-linear function of qi(t) and vi(t)

Bi(t)=V0[k1(1-qi(t))+k2(1-qi(t)vi(t))+k3(1-vi(t))] (9)

where V0 represent the fraction of venous blood (deoxygenated) in resting-state, and k1, k2, k3 are kinetic constants. We used the same parameters as in Stephan et al. [45]: τs = 0.65, τf = 0.41, τv = 0.98, τq = 0.98, κ = 0.32, E0 = 0.4, k1 = 2.77, k2 = 0.2, k3 = 0.5.

The system of differential equations (Eq (8)) was solved with the Euler method, using an integration step of 1 ms. The signals were band-pass filtered between 0.01 and 0.1 Hz with a 3rd order Bessel filter. These BOLD-like signals were used to build the functional connectivity (FC) matrices from which the subsequent analysis of functional network properties is performed using tools from graph theory.

Although the nodes consist of three neural masses, there is some evidence that the hemodynamic response is related mainly to excitatory activity [78]. In fact, some reports suggest that inhibitory activity does not trigger a measurable BOLD response, because the inhibitory connections are relatively few and their energy expenditure is lower [79]. Nevertheless, we reproduced the Fig 2 using a combined BOLD response, and we found no noticeable differences (see S6 Fig).

Global phase synchronization

As a measure of global synchronization in the EEG timescale, we calculated the Kuramoto order parameter R(t) [47] of the EEG-like signals ν(t) derived from the Jansen & Rit model. First, the raw signals were filtered with a 3rd order Bessel band-pass filter using their frequency of maximum power (usually between 4 and 10 Hz) ±3 Hz. Then, the instantaneous phase ϕ(t) was obtained with the Hilbert transform.

The global phase synchrony is computed as:

R¯=|ejϕi(t)N|t (10)

where ϕi(t) is the phase of the oscillator i over time, j=-1 the imaginary unit, |•| denotes the module, 〈〉N denotes the average over all nodes, and 〈〉t the average over time. A value of R¯ equal to 1 indicates perfect in-phase synchronization of all the set N of oscillators, while a value equal to 0 indicates total asynchrony.

Signal-to-noise ratio

We measured the average signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) over all raw signals and nodes, using the power spectral density function denoted PSD(ω). This function was calculated using the Welch’s method [80], with 20 s time windows overlapped by 50%. We excluded the 2nd to 5th harmonics [81]. For a node i, the signal power, Psignal, was measured as the area under the curve of PSD(ω) within ωi ± 1Hz. Noise power, Pnoise, corresponds to the area under the curve of PSD(ω) outside the ±1Hz window. Then, the SNR was calculated as

SNR=10log10PsignalPnoise, (11)

The SNR was computed for each node i and we reported the average over all nodes.

Functional connectivity and graph thresholding

The static Functional Connectivity (sFC) matrices were built from pairwise Pearson’s correlations of the entire BOLD-like time series. Instead of employing an absolute or proportional thresholding, we thresholded the sFC matrices using Fourier transform (FT) surrogate data [82] to avoid the problem of introducing spurious correlations [83]. The FT algorithm uses a phase randomization process to destroy pairwise correlations, preserving the spectral properties of the signals (the surrogates have the same power spectrum as the original data). We generated 500 surrogates time series of the original set of BOLD-like signals, and then built the surrogates sFC matrices. For each one of the (n2n)/2 possible connectivity pairs (with n = 90) we fitted a normal distribution of the surrogate values. Using these distributions we tested the hypothesis that a pairwise correlation is higher than chance (that is, the value is at the right of the surrogate distribution). To reject the null hypothesis, we selected a p-value equal to 0.05, and corrected for multiple comparisons with the FDR Benjamini-Hochberg procedure [84] to decrease the probability of make type I errors (false positives). The entries of the sFC matrix associated with a p-value greater than 0.05 were set to 0. The result is a thresholded, undirected, and weighted (with only positive values) sFC matrix.

Integration and segregation

Integration and segregation were evaluated over the thresholded sFC matrices. We employed the weighted versions of transitivity [85] and global efficiency [86] to measure segregation and integration, respectively. A detailed description of the metrics used can be found in Rubinov & Sporns [46]. The transitivity (similar to the average clustering coefficient) counts the fraction of triangular motifs surrounding the nodes (the equivalent of counting how many neighbors are also neighbors of each other), with the difference that it is normalized collectively. It is defined as

Tw=iN2tiwiNkiw(kiw-1), (12)

being N the set of all nodes of the network with n number of nodes, tiw the geometric average of the triangles around the node i, and kiw the node weighted degree. The supra-index w is used to refer to the weighted versions of the topological network measures. On the other hand, the global efficiency is a measure of integration based on paths over the graph: it is defined as the inverse of the average shortest path length. This metric is computed as

Ew=1niNjN,ji(dijw)-1n-1, (13)

where dijw is the shortest path between the nodes i and j.

We also calculated other two measures of integration and segregation: the participation coefficient PCw and modularity Qw, respectively, both based on the detection of the network’s communities [46]. The detection of so-called communities or network modules in the thresholded sFC matrix, was based on the Louvain’s algorithm [87, 88]. The algorithm assigns a module to each node in a way that maximizes the modularity (14). We used the weighted version of the modularity [89] defined as

Qw=1lwi,jN[wij-kiwkjwlw]δmi,mj (14)

where wij is the weight of the link between i and j, lw is the total number of weighted links of the network, mi (mj) the module of the node i (j). The Kronecker delta δmi,mj is equal to 1 when mi = mj (that is, when two nodes belongs to the same module), and 0 otherwise. Because the Louvain’s algorithm is stochastic, we employed the consensus clustering algorithm [90]. We ran the Louvain’s algorithm 200 times with the resolution parameter set to 1.0 (this parameter controls the size of the detected modules; larger values of this parameter allows the detection of smaller modules). Then, we built an agreement matrix G, in which an entry Gij indicates the proportion of partitions in which the pairs of nodes (i, j) share the same module (so, the entries of G are bounded between 0 and 1). Then, we applied an absolute threshold of 0.5 to the matrix G, and ran again the Louvain’s algorithm 200 times using G as input, producing a new consensus matrix G′. This last step was repeated until convergence to an unique partition.

Finally, we computed the weighted version of the participation coefficient [91]. This metric quantifies, for each individual node, the strength of between-module connections respect to the within-module connections, and is defined as

PCwN=1niNPCiw=1niN(1-mM(kiw(m)kiw)2) (15)

where PCiw is the weighted participation coefficient for the node i, and 〈PCwN is the average overall nodes. The functional network analysis was done in Python using the Brain Connectivity Toolbox [46].

Functional connectivity dynamics

The FCD matrix captures the evolution of FC patterns and, consequently, the dynamical richness of the network [9, 10]. We used the sliding window approach [9, 48] depicted in Fig 6. Window length was set to 100 s with a displacement of 2 s between consecutive windows (Fig 6A). The length was chosen on the basis of the lower limit of the band-pass filter (0.01 Hz), in order to minimize spurious correlations [92]. For each window, an FC matrix was calculated from the pairwise Pearson’s correlations of BOLD-like signals (neglecting negative values), thus we obtained 251 weighted and undirected FC matrices from the 600 s simulated BOLD-like signals (Fig 6B).

The upper triangle of each FC matrix is unfolded to make a vector, and the FCD is built by calculating the Clarkson angular distance λ(x,y)=12x||x-yy|| [93] between each pair of FCs (Fig 6C)

FCDij=λ(FC(ti),FC(tj)) (16)

The variance of the values in the upper triangle of the FCD, with an offset of τ = 100 s from the diagonal (e.g., the variance of the histograms of Fig 6D), is taken as a measure of dynamical richness [48].

The speed of the FCD was measured as described by Battaglia et al. [49]. We computed the histogram of FCD values through a straight line from FCD(τ, 0) to FCD(tmax, tmaxτ), with tmax = 600 s as the total time-length of the signals and τ = 100 s. The median of the histogram distribution corresponded to the typical FCD speed dtyp. Values closer to 1 indicate a constant switching of states, and values closer to 0 correspond to stable FC patterns.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Alternative measures of network segregation and integration in the (α, β) parameter space.

A) Mean participation coefficient PCw (integration) and transitivity Tw (segregation). B-C) Transitions in the α and β axes. Dashed lines represent critical transitions.

(PDF)

S2 Fig. Network features in the (α, C1) parameter space.

A) Global efficiency Ew (integration) and modularity Qw (segregation) of the graphs derived from the sFCs of the BOLD-like signals. B) Transitions in the α axis, for a fixed C1 = 0. C) Transitions in the C1 axis, for a fixed α = 0.5. Dashed lines represent critical transitions.

(PDF)

S3 Fig. Alternative measures of network segregation and integration in the (α, r0) parameter space for β = 0.4.

A-B) Mean participation coefficient PCw (integration) and transitivity Tw (segregation) with A) β = 0 and B) β = 0.4. C-D) Transitions in the α and r0 axes. Dashed lines represent critical transitions.

(PDF)

S4 Fig. Simultaneous effect of α, β and r0 in network features.

A) Global efficiency Ew (integration) and modularity Qw (segregation) of the graphs derived from the sFCs of the BOLD-like signals. B) Transitions in the α axis, for a fixed r0 = 0.5 mV−1. C) Transitions in the r0 axis, for a fixed α = 0.5. Dashed lines represent critical transitions. Both coupling parameters change in parallel following the relationship β = 0.5α.

(PDF)

S5 Fig. Effect of the input standard deviation, σ, and mean, μ, in synchronization and integration.

A-B) Average phase synchrony R¯ measured over the EEG-like signals, and global efficiency Ew computed over the sFC matrices build using the BOLD-like signals. σ and μ reduces the increment of R¯ and Ew mediated by α and β. Both coupling parameters change in parallel following the relationship β = 0.5α. C-D) Phase synchrony R¯ and global efficiency Ew as a function of α and β, for different σ (left hand) and μ (right hand) values.

(PDF)

S6 Fig. Network integration computed from mixed BOLD-like signals.

A) Global efficiency Ew computed in the entire (α, β) parameter space. BOLD-like signals were computed using only the firing rates of pyramidal neurons. B) Ew calculated starting from a summation of the BOLD-like signals simulated using the firing rates of the three neural masses: pyramidal neurons, excitatory and inhibitory interneurons. C) Difference in the global efficiency ΔEw between the two matrices in the (α, β) parameter space. Green values correspond to near-zero difference between the matrices. There is not a noticeable difference between them.

(PDF)

S7 Fig. Bifurcation analysis of the modified Jansen & Rit model and comparison to the original model.

Bifurcations of a single-node model with respect to the mean external input parameter p, at three values of β. Note that β = 0 corresponds to the original Jansen & Rit model. The value depicted in the y-axis is the variable x0. Red and black lines denote stable and unstable fixed points, respectively. Green solid lines and blue dashed lines represent stable and unstable periodic attractors, respectively (denoting the maximum and minimum values of the oscillation). At the right, sample time courses (3 seconds) of the EEG-like signal (C2 x1(t) − C4 x2(t)) at three values of p, and their corresponding power spectra below.

(PDF)

S8 Fig. Effect of neuromodulation on the mean oscillatory frequency ω.

The frequency falls in the Theta range (4-8 Hz) of the EEG spectrum in both the A (α, β) and B (α, r0) parameter spaces. In the first case we fixed r0 = 0.56 mV−1, and in the second one β = 0.4. The frequency drop off matches with phase synchronization (see Fig 5).

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We thank to Gustavo Deco who kindly provided the anatomical connectivity matrix used in the model. We also want to thank to Chiayu Chiu and Andrés Chávez for their feedback and suggestions about the manuscript.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files. The code we use to obtain our results is open source and available at https://github.com/vandal-uv/Neuromod2020.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by Fondo Nacional de Desarrollo Científico y Tecnológico - Fondecyt Grants 1181076 (to PO) and 11181072 (to RC) and the Advanced Center for Electrical and Electronic Engineering - ANID (FB0008 to PO). The Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencia de Valparaíso (CINV) is a Millenium Institute supported by the ANID grant ICN09_022. CC-O is funded by Beca Doctorado Nacional – ANID grant 2018- 21180995. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Tononi G, Sporns O, Edelman GM. A measure for brain complexity: relating functional segregation and integration in the nervous system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1994;91(11):5033–5037. 10.1073/pnas.91.11.5033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Cohen JR, D’Esposito M. The segregation and integration of distinct brain networks and their relationship to cognition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36(48):12083–12094. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2965-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Shine JM, Bissett PG, Bell PT, Koyejo O, Balsters JH, Gorgolewski KJ, et al. The dynamics of functional brain networks: integrated network states during cognitive task performance. Neuron. 2016;92(2):544–554. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Shine JM, Breakspear M, Bell PT, Martens KAE, Shine R, Koyejo O, et al. Human cognition involves the dynamic integration of neural activity and neuromodulatory systems. Nature neuroscience. 2019;22(2):289–296. 10.1038/s41593-018-0312-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Kelso JS. Multistability and metastability: understanding dynamic coordination in the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;367(1591):906–918. 10.1098/rstb.2011.0351 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Tognoli E, Kelso JS. The metastable brain. Neuron. 2014;81(1):35–48. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Sporns O. Network attributes for segregation and integration in the human brain. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2013;23(2):162–171. 10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Allen EA, Damaraju E, Plis SM, Erhardt EB, Eichele T, Calhoun VD. Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cerebral cortex. 2014;24(3):663–676. 10.1093/cercor/bhs352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Hansen EC, Battaglia D, Spiegler A, Deco G, Jirsa VK. Functional connectivity dynamics: modeling the switching behavior of the resting state. Neuroimage. 2015;105:525–535. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Cabral J, Kringelbach ML, Deco G. Functional connectivity dynamically evolves on multiple time-scales over a static structural connectome: Models and mechanisms. NeuroImage. 2017;160:84–96. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Guan S, Jiang R, Bian H, Yuan J, Xu P, Meng C, et al. The profiles of non-stationarity and non-linearity in the time series of resting-state brain networks. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2020;14:493. 10.3389/fnins.2020.00493 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Shine JM, van den Brink RL, Hernaus D, Nieuwenhuis S, Poldrack RA. Catecholaminergic manipulation alters dynamic network topology across cognitive states. Network Neuroscience. 2018;2(3):381–396. 10.1162/netn_a_00042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Shine JM. Neuromodulatory influences on integration and segregation in the brain. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2019; 10.1016/j.tics.2019.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Thiele A, Bellgrove MA. Neuromodulation of attention. Neuron. 2018;97(4):769–785. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:403–450. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Jones Hasselmo ME, Sarter M. Modes and models of forebrain cholinergic neuromodulation of cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(1):52–73. 10.1038/npp.2010.104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Marder E. Neuromodulation of neuronal circuits: back to the future. Neuron. 2012;76(1):1–11. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Kilgard MP, Merzenich MM. Cortical map reorganization enabled by nucleus basalis activity. Science. 1998;279(5357):1714–1718. 10.1126/science.279.5357.1714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Herrero JL, Roberts M, Delicato LS, Gieselmann MA, Dayan P, Thiele A. Acetylcholine contributes through muscarinic receptors to attentional modulation in V1. Nature. 2008;454(7208):1110–1114. 10.1038/nature07141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Thiele A, Herrero JL, Distler C, Hoffmann KP. Contribution of cholinergic and GABAergic mechanisms to direction tuning, discriminability, response reliability, and neuronal rate correlations in macaque middle temporal area. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32(47):16602–16615. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0554-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Fuxe K, Dahlström AB, Jonsson G, Marcellino D, Guescini M, Dam M, et al. The discovery of central monoamine neurons gave volume transmission to the wired brain. Progress in neurobiology. 2010;90(2):82–100. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Foote S, Aston-Jones G, Bloom F. Impulse activity of locus coeruleus neurons in awake rats and monkeys is a function of sensory stimulation and arousal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1980;77(5):3033–3037. 10.1073/pnas.77.5.3033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Waterhouse BD, Sessler FM, Jung-Tung C, Woodward DJ, Azizi SA, Moises HC. New evidence for a gating action of norepinephrine in central neuronal circuits of mammalian brain. Brain research bulletin. 1988;21(3):425–432. 10.1016/0361-9230(88)90154-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Waterhouse BD, Moises HC, Woodward DJ. Phasic activation of the locus coeruleus enhances responses of primary sensory cortical neurons to peripheral receptive field stimulation. Brain research. 1998;790(1-2):33–44. 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00117-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Servan-Schreiber D, Printz H, Cohen JD. A network model of catecholamine effects: gain, signal-to-noise ratio, and behavior. Science. 1990;249(4971):892–895. 10.1126/science.2392679 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Shine JM, Aburn MJ, Breakspear M, Poldrack RA. The modulation of neural gain facilitates a transition between functional segregation and integration in the brain. Elife. 2018;7:e31130. 10.7554/eLife.31130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Bentley P, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Cholinergic modulation of cognition: insights from human pharmacological functional neuroimaging. Progress in neurobiology. 2011;94(4):360–388. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.06.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Picciotto MR, Higley MJ, Mineur YS. Acetylcholine as a neuromodulator: cholinergic signaling shapes nervous system function and behavior. Neuron. 2012;76(1):116–129. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Hasselmo M, Giocomo L. Cholinergic modulation of cortical function. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience. 2006;30(1-2):133. 10.1385/JMN:30:1:133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Kruglikov I, Rudy B. Perisomatic GABA release and thalamocortical integration onto neocortical excitatory cells are regulated by neuromodulators. Neuron. 2008;58(6):911–924. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Poorthuis RB, Bloem B, Schak B, Wester J, de Kock CP, Mansvelder HD. Layer-specific modulation of the prefrontal cortex by nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. Cerebral cortex. 2013;23(1):148–161. 10.1093/cercor/bhr390 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Fanselow EE, Richardson KA, Connors BW. Selective, state-dependent activation of somatostatin-expressing inhibitory interneurons in mouse neocortex. Journal of neurophysiology. 2008;100(5):2640–2652. 10.1152/jn.90691.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Arroyo S, Bennett C, Aziz D, Brown SP, Hestrin S. Prolonged disynaptic inhibition in the cortex mediated by slow, non-α7 nicotinic excitation of a specific subset of cortical interneurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32(11):3859–3864. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0115-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Kimura F. Cholinergic modulation of cortical function: a hypothetical role in shifting the dynamics in cortical network. Neuroscience research. 2000;38(1):19–26. 10.1016/S0168-0102(00)00151-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Jansen BH, Zouridakis G, Brandt ME. A neurophysiologically-based mathematical model of flash visual evoked potentials. Biological cybernetics. 1993;68(3):275–283. 10.1007/BF00224863 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Jansen BH, Rit VG. Electroencephalogram and visual evoked potential generation in a mathematical model of coupled cortical columns. Biological cybernetics. 1995;73(4):357–366. 10.1007/BF00199471 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Wylie KP, Rojas DC, Tanabe J, Martin LF, Tregellas JR. Nicotine increases brain functional network efficiency. Neuroimage. 2012;63(1):73–80. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Gießing C, Thiel CM, Alexander-Bloch AF, Patel AX, Bullmore ET. Human brain functional network changes associated with enhanced and impaired attentional task performance. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(14):5903–5914. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4854-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Silberberg G, Markram H. Disynaptic inhibition between neocortical pyramidal cells mediated by Martinotti cells. Neuron. 2007;53(5):735–746. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Fino E, Packer AM, Yuste R. The logic of inhibitory connectivity in the neocortex. The Neuroscientist. 2013;19(3):228–237. 10.1177/1073858412456743 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Deco G, Cruzat J, Cabral J, Knudsen GM, Carhart-Harris RL, Whybrow PC, et al. Whole-brain multimodal neuroimaging model using serotonin receptor maps explains non-linear functional effects of LSD. Current biology. 2018;28(19):3065–3074. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15(1):273–289. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Gilbert CD, Hirsch JA, Wiesel TN. Lateral interactions in visual cortex. In: Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology. vol. 55. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press; 1990. p. 663–677. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 44. McGuire BA, Gilbert CD, Rivlin PK, Wiesel TN. Targets of horizontal connections in macaque primary visual cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1991;305(3):370–392. 10.1002/cne.903050303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Stephan KE, Weiskopf N, Drysdale PM, Robinson PA, Friston KJ. Comparing hemodynamic models with DCM. Neuroimage. 2007;38(3):387–401. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2010;52(3):1059–1069. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Acebrón JA, Bonilla LL, Vicente CJP, Ritort F, Spigler R. The Kuramoto model: A simple paradigm for synchronization phenomena. Reviews of modern physics. 2005;77(1):137. 10.1103/RevModPhys.77.137 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Orio P, Gatica M, Herzog R, Maidana JP, Castro S, Xu K. Chaos versus noise as drivers of multistability in neural networks. Chaos: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Nonlinear Science. 2018;28(10):106321. 10.1063/1.5043447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Battaglia D, Boudou T, Hansen EC, Lombardo D, Chettouf S, Daffertshofer A, et al. Dynamic Functional Connectivity between Order and Randomness and its Evolution across the Human Adult Lifespan. Neuroimage. 2020; 222:117156. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Xiang Z, Huguenard JR, Prince DA. Cholinergic switching within neocortical inhibitory networks. Science. 1998;281(5379):985–988. 10.1126/science.281.5379.985 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Deco G, Ponce-Alvarez A, Hagmann P, Romani GL, Mantini D, Corbetta M. How local excitation–inhibition ratio impacts the whole brain dynamics. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34(23):7886–7898. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5068-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Abeysuriya RG, Hadida J, Sotiropoulos SN, Jbabdi S, Becker R, Hunt BA, et al. A biophysical model of dynamic balancing of excitation and inhibition in fast oscillatory large-scale networks. PLoS computational biology. 2018;14(2):e1006007. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Lord LD, Stevner AB, Deco G, Kringelbach ML. Understanding principles of integration and segregation using whole-brain computational connectomics: implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences. 2017;375(2096):20160283. 10.1098/rsta.2016.0283 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Spiegler A, Kiebel SJ, Atay FM, Knösche TR. Bifurcation analysis of neural mass models: Impact of extrinsic inputs and dendritic time constants. NeuroImage. 2010;52(3):1041–1058. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Shafiei G, Zeighami Y, Clark CA, Coull JT, Nagano-Saito A, Leyton M, et al. Dopamine signaling modulates the stability and integration of intrinsic brain networks. Cerebral Cortex. 2019;29(1):397–409. 10.1093/cercor/bhy264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Scharinger C, Soutschek A, Schubert T, Gerjets P. Comparison of the working memory load in n-back and working memory span tasks by means of EEG frequency band power and P300 amplitude. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2017;11:6. 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Harmony T. The functional significance of delta oscillations in cognitive processing. Frontiers in integrative neuroscience. 2013;7:83. 10.3389/fnint.2013.00083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Nordenfelt A, Wagemakers A, Sanjuán MA. Frequency dispersion in the time-delayed Kuramoto model. Physical Review E. 2014;89(3):032905. 10.1103/PhysRevE.89.032905 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Lea-Carnall CA, Montemurro MA, Trujillo-Barreto NJ, Parkes LM, El-Deredy W. Cortical resonance frequencies emerge from network size and connectivity. PLoS computational biology. 2016;12(2). 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004740 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Valentine G, Sofuoglu M. Cognitive effects of nicotine: recent progress. Current neuropharmacology. 2018;16(4):403–414. 10.2174/1570159X15666171103152136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Shen EH, Overly CC, Jones AR. The Allen Human Brain Atlas: comprehensive gene expression mapping of the human brain. Trends in neurosciences. 2012;35(12):711–714. 10.1016/j.tins.2012.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Dorocic IP, Fürth D, Xuan Y, Johansson Y, Pozzi L, Silberberg G, et al. A whole-brain atlas of inputs to serotonergic neurons of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei. Neuron. 2014;83(3):663–678. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Li X, Yu B, Sun Q, Zhang Y, Ren M, Zhang X, et al. Generation of a whole-brain atlas for the cholinergic system and mesoscopic projectome analysis of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2018;115(2):415–420. 10.1073/pnas.1703601115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Kringelbach ML, Cruzat J, Cabral J, Knudsen GM, Carhart-Harris R, Whybrow PC, et al. Dynamic coupling of whole-brain neuronal and neurotransmitter systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2020;117(17):9566–9576. 10.1073/pnas.1921475117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Wendling F, Bartolomei F, Bellanger J, Chauvel P. Epileptic fast activity can be explained by a model of impaired GABAergic dendritic inhibition. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;15(9):1499–1508. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01985.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Rosas FE, Mediano PA, Gastpar M, Jensen HJ. Quantifying high-order interdependencies via multivariate extensions of the mutual information. Physical Review E. 2019;100(3):032305. 10.1103/PhysRevE.100.032305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Gatica M, Cofre R, Mediano PA, Rosas FE, Orio P, Diez I, et al. High-order interdependencies in the aging brain. bioRxiv. 2020; 10.1101/2020.03.17.995886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Petri G, Expert P, Turkheimer F, Carhart-Harris R, Nutt D, Hellyer PJ, et al. Homological scaffolds of brain functional networks. Journal of The Royal Society Interface. 2014;11(101):20140873. 10.1098/rsif.2014.0873 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Lord LD, Expert P, Fernandes HM, Petri G, Van Hartevelt TJ, Vaccarino F, et al. Insights into brain architectures from the homological scaffolds of functional connectivity networks. Frontiers in systems neuroscience. 2016;10:85. 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00085 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Geniesse C, Sporns O, Petri G, Saggar M. Generating dynamical neuroimaging spatiotemporal representations (DyNeuSR) using topological data analysis. Network Neuroscience. 2019;3(3):763–778. 10.1162/netn_a_00093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Griffa A, Baumann PS, Thiran JP, Hagmann P. Structural connectomics in brain diseases. Neuroimage. 2013;80:515–526. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Brier MR, Thomas JB, Fagan AM, Hassenstab J, Holtzman DM, Benzinger TL, et al. Functional connectivity and graph theory in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of aging. 2014;35(4):757–768. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.10.081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. delEtoile J, Adeli H. Graph theory and brain connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. The Neuroscientist. 2017;23(6):616–626. 10.1177/1073858417702621 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Chen Y, Huang X, Wu M, Li K, Hu X, Jiang P, et al. Disrupted brain functional networks in drug-naïve children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder assessed using graph theory analysis. Human brain mapping. 2019;40(17):4877–4887. 10.1002/hbm.24743 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Rocha RP, Koçillari L, Suweis S, Corbetta M, Maritan A. Homeostatic plasticity and emergence of functional networks in a whole-brain model at criticality. Scientific reports. 2018;8(1):1–15. 10.1038/s41598-018-33923-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Branco T, Häusser M. Synaptic integration gradients in single cortical pyramidal cell dendrites. Neuron. 2011;69(5):885–892. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Birn RM, Molloy EK, Patriat R, Parker T, Meier TB, Kirk GR, et al. The effect of scan length on the reliability of resting-state fMRI connectivity estimates. Neuroimage. 2013;83:550–558. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Buxton RB, Uludağ K, Dubowitz DJ, Liu TT. Modeling the hemodynamic response to brain activation. Neuroimage. 2004;23:S220–S233. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Waldvogel D, van Gelderen P, Muellbacher W, Ziemann U, Immisch I, Hallett M. The relative metabolic demand of inhibition and excitation. Nature. 2000;406(6799):995–998. 10.1038/35023171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Welch P. The use of fast Fourier transform for the estimation of power spectra: a method based on time averaging over short, modified periodograms. IEEE Transactions on audio and electroacoustics. 1967;15(2):70–73. 10.1109/TAU.1967.1161901 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Schultz SR. Signal-to-noise ratio in neuroscience. Scholarpedia. 2007;2(6):2046. 10.4249/scholarpedia.2046 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Lancaster G, Iatsenko D, Pidde A, Ticcinelli V, Stefanovska A. Surrogate data for hypothesis testing of physical systems. Physics Reports. 2018;748:1–60. 10.1016/j.physrep.2018.06.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Fornito A, Zalesky A, Breakspear M. Graph analysis of the human connectome: promise, progress, and pitfalls. Neuroimage. 2013;80:426–444. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal statistical society: series B (Methodological). 1995;57(1):289–300. 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Newman ME. The structure and function of complex networks. SIAM review. 2003;45(2):167–256. 10.1137/S003614450342480 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Latora V, Marchiori M. Efficient behavior of small-world networks. Physical review letters. 2001;87(19):198701. 10.1103/PhysRevLett.87.198701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Newman ME. Modularity and community structure in networks. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences. 2006;103(23):8577–8582. 10.1073/pnas.0601602103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Blondel VD, Guillaume JL, Lambiotte R, Lefebvre E. Fast unfolding of communities in large networks. Journal of statistical mechanics: theory and experiment. 2008;2008(10):P10008. 10.1088/1742-5468/2008/10/P10008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Newman ME. Analysis of weighted networks. Physical review E. 2004;70(5):056131. 10.1103/PhysRevE.70.056131 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Lancichinetti A, Fortunato S. Consensus clustering in complex networks. Scientific reports. 2012;2:336. 10.1038/srep00336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Guimera R, Amaral LAN. Cartography of complex networks: modules and universal roles. Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment. 2005;2005(02):P02001. 10.1088/1742-5468/2005/02/P02001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Leonardi N, Van De Ville D. On spurious and real fluctuations of dynamic functional connectivity during rest. Neuroimage. 2015;104:430–436. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Clarkson JA. Uniformly convex spaces. Transactions of the American Mathematical Society. 1936;40(3):396–414. 10.1090/S0002-9947-1936-1501880-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008737.r001

Decision Letter 0

Daniele Marinazzo

28 Oct 2020

Dear Mr. Orio,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Cholinergic neuromodulation of inhibitory interneurons facilitates functional integration in whole-brain models" for consideration at PLOS Computational Biology.

As with all papers reviewed by the journal, your manuscript was reviewed by members of the editorial board and by several independent reviewers. In light of the reviews (below this email), we would like to invite the resubmission of a significantly-revised version that takes into account the reviewers' comments.

The major points to address range from the situation in the state of the art, to the nature and clarity of the proposed approach, to the application and implications, so we suggest extra care in organically addressing all of them.

We cannot make any decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is also likely to be sent to reviewers for further evaluation.

When you are ready to resubmit, please upload the following:

[1] A letter containing a detailed list of your responses to the review comments and a description of the changes you have made in the manuscript. Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

[2] Two versions of the revised manuscript: one with either highlights or tracked changes denoting where the text has been changed; the other a clean version (uploaded as the manuscript file).

Important additional instructions are given below your reviewer comments.

Please prepare and submit your revised manuscript within 60 days. If you anticipate any delay, please let us know the expected resubmission date by replying to this email. Please note that revised manuscripts received after the 60-day due date may require evaluation and peer review similar to newly submitted manuscripts.

Thank you again for your submission. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive so far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Daniele Marinazzo

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Daniele Marinazzo

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: Thank you for inviting me to review this manuscript by Coronel-Oliveros and colleagues, in which the authors adapt a Jansen-Rit neuronal model to replicate and extend previous work relating the ascending neuromodulatory arousal system to network-level topological characteristics and temporal signatures of neural activity.

• Can the authors please discuss what the ‘excitatory interneuron’ population represents in the Jansen-Rit model. The term ‘interneuron’ is typically used to describe locally-projecting GABAergic neurons (doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-070918-050421). Although some of these neurons, by virtue of their inhibitory projections to other GABAergic neurons, are thought to disinhibit excitatory pyramidal neurons (e.g., doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0508-y), it is hard to know whether this is the population the authors are referring to. Alternatively, the excitatory interneuron population could reflect local (i.e., within column) recurrent activity, however this would imply a different set of properties (e.g., time-scales) and responsiveness (or not) to different classes of neuromodulatory neurotransmitters (see next point).

• Along these lines, I worry that the link between the cholinergic system and the inhibitory connection introduced in their model may not be as specific as the authors hope. Importantly, this depends on precisely how the authors (and others) conceptualize the excitatory interneuron population. If they are conceptualizing EI as VIP+ interneurons, then these cells have demonstrated responsivity to serotonin (doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1869-10.2010), suggesting that the effects identified by the authors are not specific to acetyl choline. If the effects of EI are presumed to relate to recurrent pyramidal neurons, these connections could take the form of many different classes of cells (doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0685-8)

• Have the authors tested the stability/fit of their model following the addition of the new inhibitory connection? It’s possible that, by adding this gain, the authors have fundamentally altered the fit to LFP data from the original study. This could potentially explain why the authors required different parameter combinations (e.g., Beta = 0.4 in Fig 4B) to recover the original results from Shine et al (2018).

Reviewer #2: The paper by Coronel-Oliveros et al. describes the effect of including local inhibitory circuits and their cholinergic modulation on segregation/integration balance in a modified Jansen-Rit model. These newly added local inhibitory coupling between interneurons is a variation of modelling inhibition-mediated control of the E/I balance which is an interesting approach to modelling neuromodulatory systems.

General comment:

While the methods are profound, the graphs are of a high quality and the language of the manuscript is very understandable, the manuscript would benefit from clearly stating the essential message and sticking to its focus by restricting the number of exploratory analyses. In particular, while the introduction clearly states the interest in integration and segregation as a function of cholinergic action in inhibitory interneurons, also parameters such as Kuramoto order parameter, signal-to-noise-ratio, regularity, static FC, FCD, FCD speed, multistability (not even discussed in the methods), phase synchrony, mean oscillatory frequency and many more have been added. To me, the advantage of so many analyses and a wealth of companyingvery similar figures 2-5, 7 is not clear. Rather

this approach seems like a rather exploratory analysis of all these parameters (that were taken from various computational neuroscience papers) without a clear focus and this approach not only dilutes the mean focus of the paper, makes all these values and their dependence on alpha and beta much harder to interpret. The authors should restrict the parameters to those who answer the initial question of the manuscript and also rewrite the results and discussion section with a clearer focus to make their message more understandable. If more parameters than the original ones (segregation, integration) are chosen, it should be stated in the introduction on how they contribute to answering the original question.

Detailed comments:

The results section includes many interpretations of the analyses (e.g. line 167, 161, 184, 172-183, etc). The authors should move these interpretations to the discussion section to make the results section more concise.

L. 162/203/etc: at times, different values when fixing alpha and beta were chosen. What was the rationale between switching the fixed parameters? It would make sense to stick with the same fixed parameters across the different analyses and explain why these exact values were chosen.

L. 498: what is the advantage to simulating a BOLD signal for these analyses? Why not stick with the EEG signal?

Fig. 3: is along the lines of my first comment. I do not see the main message of this figure and how it contributes to answering the original question

Fig 1 and 4 seem almost redundant to me- in my view, the manuscript would benefit from reducing the number of figures.

L. 209: with a wealth of parameters, the critical boundaries

Discussion:

When reading the discussion, I found it hard to understand the main focus of it- partly it is reads like an enumeration of ideas that are only partly connected. As I said in my first comment, the manuscript would benefit from a more precise and restricted analysis and in a similar fashion from a more focussed discussion focussing on the original question.

L. 298: Please sum up the main results in the first section of the discussion

L. 309: the referenced papers (11,19) do not include any analyses of integration. Which experiments do you mean exactly and what did they really show? So far, I have only seen this relationship in whole-brain models only. Please add experimental papers that were dealing with neuromodulation and segregation/integration.

L. 332: which results back that up? I missed the inverted U across the large amount of figures. Here, I again reiterate to reduce the number of figures according to your main question and focus on the figures with the main message.

L. 402: here two more parameters are introduced that should be removed from the manuscript to sharpen the focus on the original question

Code/Data availability:

The authors provided a github repo with the accompanying code to reproduce the simulations. While I did not run the simulations myself, I found the code to be very well written and understandable and well documented. In addition to the provided codes, it would be good to include the measures of integration and segregation into the code as these are the main variables of interest in the paper.

Reviewer #3: The authors Coronel-Oliveros, Cofré, and Orio, performed a parameter exploration of a whole-brain network model utilizing local dynamics from a neural mass model to describe the effects of neuromodulatory systems and functional segregation and integration in the brain on the source network level and the level of BOLD and EEG. The presented study heavily relies on Shine et al., 2019, and the modeling work Shine et al., 2018. The authors extended the modeling work of Shine et al., 2018 by using a different local dynamic model. The authors considered two neuromodulatory systems, the cholinergic and the noradrenergic system. Both are assumed to act uniformly in the brain at the level of cortical columns. The cholinergic system is also considered to modulate the connectivity via white-matter fiber tracts. Both systems are assumed to act independently and on a slower time scale than the local dynamics. In a first modeling study, the authors systematically varied the level of local connectivity of inhibitory to excitatory interneurons and the level of long-range connectivity and assessed integration and segregation by graph-theoretic measures. In a successive study, the authors performed a similar analysis. They varied the level of long-range connectivity and the variance of firing thresholds for fixed connectivity levels from inhibitory to excitatory interneurons. The main result is that the cholinergic system action on both the long-range connectivity and the inhibition of excitatory interneurons is needed to shift from an unsynchronized regime towards a coherent activity (integrated). The model predicts that the projection of inhibitory to excitatory interneurons is important for controlling the dynamics of a brain area.

I appreciate that kind of modeling work. However, the paper in the present version misses a proper description of observed phenomena associated with neuromodulation through the cholinergic and the noradrenergic system. Effects are often too vaguely described, and it is not clear how they are reflected in brain signals such as EEG and BOLD. I also miss a convincing motivation for the used model. For instance, the term neuromodulation repeatedly appears in the text, but the associated parameters are constants and do not change with time in the model. For the reader, it is important to know how constant model parameters and neuromodulation go together. The authors show the effect of the model parameters on the graph-theoretic measures (efficiency and modularity) and dynamic functional connectivity to assess integration and segregation of functional network states. However, the authors do not show and mention any particular functional networks. I am curious to see the occurring networks and how meaningful they are. I unfortunately cannot recommend the manuscript in its current stage for publishing in Plos-CB.

**Major comments

My reservations concern the model and the description of the neuromodulation systems, most of which can be addressed by improving and elaborating the text's description.

The description of the cholinergic and the noradrenergic systems should be clearer and more consistent throughout the paper. On the one hand, the authors should elaborate more on the physiological aspect - why these systems are so important? On the other hand, the authors should better motivate neuromodulation's modeled action in the local dynamic model.

From the author's description of the biophysical mechanisms, I could imagine other implementations for the action of both systems, for example,

*the cholinergic system :

"lines 56/57, increasing the excitability of pyramidal neurons" could be modeled by lowering firing threshold theta of pyramidal cells and by increasing PSP gain, that is, C_2, C_4, and C at pyramidal cells. Why have the authors decided to scale the input from other brain areas to describe pyramidal cells' increased excitability? In the model, input from other brain areas is linear in the PSP at pyramidal cells. I agree that "lines 70/71: pyramidal neurons become more responsive to stimulus from other distant regions respect to the stimulus of its own cortical column." However, is that equivalent to "increasing the excitability of pyramidal neurons"? Please elaborate.

"Lines 67/68, enhancing the activity and firing rates of dendritic-targeting GABAergic interneurons" should be modeled by beta*x_2(t) at excitatory interneurons as well as pyramidal cells. Why is the presented model beta*x_2(t) affecting excitatory interneurons only and not the inhibitory projections onto pyramidal cells?

*the noradrenergic system :

“Increases responsiveness to input-driven activity respect to spontaneous activity and filters out noise.” In my opinion, that action is better represented in the model by the scaling alpha of the connections between brain areas, also because the "lines 51/52: the effect is more pronounced between distant brain regions." How do the authors relate this noradrenergic effect to a slope change in converting postsynaptic potentials to the firing rate? A flat slope allows for a linear transfer of potential dynamics into rate dynamics. A steep transfer function restricts the dynamic range of the conversion. Therefore, a neural mass is more likely to be saturated: the saturated unexcited or in the saturated excited state. In both states, the neural mass is less sensitive. In general, the slope reflects the variance of the firing thresholds theta within a neural mass. That's why I am curious to know why the authors have decided to alter the slope of potential-to-rate function for all neural masses? Please explain.

The authors should elaborate on the model decision. Most of the cholinergic neurotransmission is known on the level of neurons but the authors used a neural mass model instead of a neuronal model. Neurotransmitters are not directly implemented in neural mass models. The associated neurotransmitters do act on different time scales (see, Shine, 2019). What are the neurotransmitters that potentially drive the constant level changes in the presented model. The authors should answer the question of why the level of neural masses and large-scale brain networks is appropriate for studying neuromodulation and functional integration and segregation in general? For instance, it is unclear how connectivity speed is derived from the human connectomes. Here, I guess, the authors confused transmission delay/time with transmission speed. Please clarify and describe how to derive values with unit 1/seconds from a distance (which distance measure was used?).

It is also unclear why the long-range connectivity speed (delay) affects the characteristic time constant of (dendritic) excitatory postsynaptic potentials at pyramidal cells? The impulse response functions h_E(t) and h_I(t) are properties of local neural masses such as the pyramidal cells. In contrast, long-range connectivity is a network property. The characteristic time constant of postsynaptic potentials differs dependent on the target of synapses on the dendrite. Studies on the single pyramidal cells show that inhibitory interneurons target closer to the soma and excitatory interneurons more distal. Excitatory synapses from more distant areas target more distal and show a distinct characteristic time constant in the postsynaptic potentials. So I agree to the extent that long-range input is integrated with a different time, but I do not see the point of having a different time constant for different lengths of long-range connections. Please elaborate. Time delays tau would read x_{3,ij}(t-tau_{ij}) in the equation system (1).

What are the model assumptions? The modulatory systems do show spatial organization (e.g., https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1703601115). Is that an approximation? I suggest adding paragraphs discussing model assumptions, expectations, data descriptions, predictions, and how to test these.

**My specific comments (reading the manuscript from the beginning to the end).

Line 3: What is an optimal behavioral outcome?

Line 5: This statement should be softened. There are also other potential candidates for describing state changes (multistability dynamics, structured flow on manifolds, etc.).

Line 8: The point that "segregation/integration balance is impaired in several neuropsychiatric disorders" should be discussed in more detail in the main text. How is such an impairment of segregation/integration balance reflected in brain signals? What are the relevant disorders?

Line 35: Please provide a reference for the "non-stationarity" of functional connectivity. Are fluctuations at rest non-stationary or non-linear? See, for example, https://dx.doi.org/10.3389%2Ffnins.2020.00493

Line 40: "Neuromodulatory systems provide a biophysical mechanism that enhances the dynamical flexibility." What are these systems? Please provide examples? It appears to be a category of several systems that are capable of modulating neurons. Also, a definition of "dynamical flexibility" is missing.

Line 44: "Indeed, the cholinergic system increments " That is one specific neuromodulatory system. It reads like there is no other. Please summarise for the reader what the "cholinergic system" is and elaborate on the neurophysical role of this system and its elements.

Line 49: What is the "noradrenergic system"? Please elaborate.

Line 50: "input-driven activity" What is the input? Do you mean stimuli like visual stimuli? Do you mean any input that a neural population receives other than its intrinsic "spontaneous activity"? Please clarify.

Line 67: Please clarify the difference between "activity and firing rates"?

Lines 67 to 71: ".. and second, enhancing the activity and firing rates of dendritic-targeting GABAergic interneurons, an effect that promotes intra-columnar inhibition, reducing the local excitatory feedback to pyramidal neurons [23,26,27]. This reads like "reducing the local excitatory feedback to pyramidal neurons" is a necessary reaction of the increased "intra-columnar inhibition." That is not necessarily the case because the pyramidal cells are also affected by intra-columnar inhibition.

Lines 89 to 91: “.. "excitatory gain,” which increases the inter-columnar coupling. This gain mechanism is mediated by the action of the cholinergic system in pyramidal neurons, principally but not exclusively, and increments pyramidal excitability [10, 11, 22]." Is it not the noradrenergic system that acts on a large-scale between brain areas, as mentioned before?

Lines 95 to 99: "Finally, we incorporated a “filter gain,” that increments the pyramidal neurons sigmoid function slope [11]. The noradrenergic system mediates this last gain mechanism; it acts as a filter, decreasing (increasing) the responsivity to weak (strong) stimuli [15,17], boosting signal-to-noise ratio and promoting integration [10]." The described actions are local and equal for all neural masses. Still, the effect is described to be long-range "lines 51 to 52: This effect is more pronounced between distant brain regions, in which structural connectivity is relatively low, promoting functional integration." Please clarify.

Lines 120/121: The time delays are not defined in the Materials and Methods section. If time delays are defined by the distance between brain areas divided by a speed, please clarify and discuss the assumed speed (is it a spatially invariant constant). What distance measure was applied (Euclidean as a lower bound proxy or mean streamline length?). Please elaborate.

Page 4, Fig. 1. "The cholinergic system has a multiplicative effect on the sigmoid function. α amplifies pyramidal neurons' response to other columns’ input" What do the authors mean exactly with multiplicative effect on the sigmoid function? Please explain in the main text? Please clarify "response"? Do the authors refer to postsynaptic potentials or firing rate?

Lines 127 to 139: The authors should emphasize the model parameters and that these are constant levels for each simulation. I understand the presented model in that way that there is no neuromodulation. The model parameters alpha, beta, and r_0 are constants and are no functions of time as readers might expect from "neuromodulation." The authors should highlight time scales, separate them, and why and under which circumstances the systematic exploration of constant parameters matters. Because this is so important for the modeling work, this should be mentioned and discussed at several stages in the manuscript.

Lines 242 to 244: There is a difference between noise and chaos in the model. A noise process drives the model with predefined moments. Chaos can occur due to the model's complexity and the coupling in the network (see https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002298 and https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.015). Although the applied measure does not distinguish between noise and chaos, the system's ability to show deterministic chaos should be discussed. Whereas the noise process represents an additional dimension and something 'unknown' extrinsic, the deterministic chaos is intrinsic and produced and maintained by the system itself.

Lines 259 to 297: The authors have to define the term criticality? Is it a statistical term, or does it correspond to deterministic mechanisms such as bifurcations that occur in the local dynamic model? Please elaborate.

Line 299 to 306: The authors refer to experimental findings based on the action of nicotinic acetylcholine (20,23,27) and somatostatin receptors (26) on spiking single neurons. How do these electrophysiological findings translate into the hypothesis that "cholinergic neuromodulation of the inhibitory interneurons (that suppresses the local 300 excitatory feedback to pyramidal neurons) facilitates functional integration?" Moreover, how can the utilized mesoscopic - large-scale level of neural masses and long-range connectivity help test the hypothesis. Why do the authors use forward models for EEG and BOLD? Do EEG and BOLD data exist supporting the hypothesis linked to the electrophysiological findings?

Line 307 to 310: The references 11 and 19 are reviews, so I wonder which extend the presented model can explain the, in refs 11 and 19, discussed experimental data. Does the model explain more than the already described and modeled inverted U-shape (10,18)?

Lines 317/18: Again, the modes as presented do not include time delays. In the model, the transmission times of long-range connections determine the local characteristic time scale of the excitatory postsynaptic potentials at the receiving pyramidal neurons. Here, the authors should give motivation for that implementation in the model. To me, it does not sound biophysically plausible.

Lines 449: How important are the mean and variance of the noise process for the results? What is the effect of noise?

Lines 465 to 468: Do the authors really mean speed here? The physical SI-unit for speed is meter/second. So I wonder, how is the speed (m/s) derived from the connectome? Usually, the distance is decided by a constant speed to approximate transmission delays. However, to include transmission times as characteristic time constants in the ODEs is also not correct as these are two different things. The characteristic times in the ODEs, in fact, h_{E, I}(t) represent local properties of the postsynaptic responses to synaptic input and should not vary for different incoming connections. Please elaborate. These points need to be clarified. What is the interpretation of the distances between brain areas here? Are we talking about Euclidean distances or mean streamline lengths between brain regions?

Pages 13- The equations in the Materials and Methods section should be consistently numbered. In the presented manuscript are 17 equations, but only three equations have numbers.

Page 13, equation (1):

There are four sets of 2nd-order ODEs. For better understanding, it is worth describing their function.

I. The first two equations are for the excitatory projections of pyramidal cells onto both interneurons.

II. The second pair of equations is for the excitatory projections of excitatory interneurons onto pyramidal cells. Here we see, that the external input is assumed to be excitatory and share the characteristic time constant of intra-areal excitatory projections at the pyramidal cells.

III. The third pair of equations is for the inhibitory projections of inhibitory interneurons onto pyramidal cells.

IV. The fourth pair is very similar to the I pair of equations only differs in the scales and is meant to explain the excitatory long-range projections of pyramidal cells in distant areas onto target pyramidal cells. Here the time constants depend on the length of incoming connections, for which an explanation is missing. Please elaborate. The notation of the incoming activity to y_0 and y_3 are identical (within the sigmoids). This becomes circular with inserting z_i(t) (the unnumbered equation below). I guess, the input to y_3 should read ( C_2 x_{1,j} - C_4 x_{2,j} + C alpha z_j ) because the sigmoid looks backward into the source. That's why the equation for the average input should also be adapted to, for instance, z_{a,b} = sum^{n}_{b=1} M_{ab} x_{3,ab}, where a,b are indices of brain areas and b is the source whereas a is the target index.

The authors should elaborate on the fact that the connectivity weights are normalized individually per receiving brain area. Why is that? The equation for the average input can be simplified M_{ab} = Mij/sum_{j} (M{ij), where Mij is the weight as presented in the manuscript. An overall normalization by a scalar uniformly applied to all entries, for instance, the maximum in-strength sounds more plausible. By using an input wise normalization, the relative weights between receiving brain areas are lost.

The equations show that the inhibitory interneurons have two targets: the pyramidal cells and the excitatory interneurons. The authors motivated the scaling of inhibitory activity as neuromodulation. Why is it then that only the inhibitory postsynaptic potential at the excitatory interneurons are scales but not the inhibitory postsynaptic potential at the pyramidal cells? However, the source of activity is identical? This modeling choice must be better motivated.

Line 473: the maximum firing rate zeta should be in 1/s - it's a rate, not a frequency.

Lines 475 to 477: Why is only the firing rate of pyramidal cells used to calculate BOLD? Mainly, the pyramidal cells' postsynaptic potential is reflected in M/EEG because of the number of pyramidal cells and their arrangement. However, that does not apply to BOLD. Here all neural masses contribute.

**********

Have all data underlying the figures and results presented in the manuscript been provided?

Large-scale datasets should be made available via a public repository as described in the PLOS Computational Biology data availability policy, and numerical data that underlies graphs or summary statistics should be provided in spreadsheet form as supporting information.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes: Andreas Spiegler

Figure Files:

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org.

Data Requirements:

Please note that, as a condition of publication, PLOS' data policy requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions outlined in your manuscript. Data must be deposited in an appropriate repository, included within the body of the manuscript, or uploaded as supporting information. This includes all numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.. For an example in PLOS Biology see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5.

Reproducibility:

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, PLOS recommends that you deposit laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions, please see http://journals.plos.org/compbiol/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008737.r003

Decision Letter 1

Daniele Marinazzo

19 Jan 2021

Dear Mr. Orio,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Cholinergic neuromodulation of inhibitory interneurons facilitates functional integration in whole-brain models" for consideration at PLOS Computational Biology. As with all papers reviewed by the journal, your manuscript was reviewed by members of the editorial board and by several independent reviewers. The reviewers appreciated the attention to an important topic. Based on the reviews, we are likely to accept this manuscript for publication, providing that you modify the manuscript according to the review recommendations.

Please prepare and submit your revised manuscript within 30 days. If you anticipate any delay, please let us know the expected resubmission date by replying to this email. 

When you are ready to resubmit, please upload the following:

[1] A letter containing a detailed list of your responses to all review comments, and a description of the changes you have made in the manuscript. Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out

[2] Two versions of the revised manuscript: one with either highlights or tracked changes denoting where the text has been changed; the other a clean version (uploaded as the manuscript file).

Important additional instructions are given below your reviewer comments.

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive so far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Daniele Marinazzo

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************

A link appears below if there are any accompanying review attachments. If you believe any reviews to be missing, please contact ploscompbiol@plos.org immediately:

[LINK]

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Authors:

Please note here if the review is uploaded as an attachment.

Reviewer #1: The authors have adequately addressed my concerns.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for inviting me again to review this manuscript on neuromodulatory systems and their relationship with segregation and integration. I read the manuscript in great detail and I was very pleased with the changes, esp. with the new version of the discussion and I enjoyed re-reading the manuscript, this time with a better understanding of the different hypotheses and arguments raised by the authors. My major concern, the lack of scope, has been addressed throughout the manuscript, so that I was able to easily follow the leitmotif provided by the authors. I am very pleased that my concerns and suggestions have been addressed and overall I feel that the manuscript has improved substantially after the revision. I only have a few minor remarks (the line count refers to the version that highlights the changes):

Introduction:

I found the introduction, in comparison to the overall length of the manuscript, a little bit lengthy. Making it more concise could improve the readability of the article.

l. 119: Which experimental findings are being referred to? Later in the manuscript the authors rather rague that they want to provide two time scales- I find this argument more convicing

Fig.1: It would make sense to the modification of the model more visible (e.g. using a color), to help the reader to directly see the changes

Fig. 3: It would be great to associate the red circles to the letters within the picture (e.g. B) to faster see which circle relates to which graph

l.428- As this analysis is based on a simulation, I would rather not talk about "experiment"

l.598- It might be more advantageous for the authors' argument to name diseases with a known cholinergic deficit (e.g. Alzheimer's or Parkinson's disease dementia) and noradrenergic deficit (e.g. ADHD)

Discussion:

I especially enjoyed reading the new version of the discussion

**********

Have all data underlying the figures and results presented in the manuscript been provided?

Large-scale datasets should be made available via a public repository as described in the PLOS Computational Biology data availability policy, and numerical data that underlies graphs or summary statistics should be provided in spreadsheet form as supporting information.

Reviewer #1: None

Reviewer #2: No: 

**********

PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Figure Files:

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org.

Data Requirements:

Please note that, as a condition of publication, PLOS' data policy requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions outlined in your manuscript. Data must be deposited in an appropriate repository, included within the body of the manuscript, or uploaded as supporting information. This includes all numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.. For an example in PLOS Biology see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5.

Reproducibility:

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, PLOS recommends that you deposit laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see http://journals.plos.org/ploscompbiol/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008737.r005

Decision Letter 2

Daniele Marinazzo

25 Jan 2021

Dear Mr. Orio,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript 'Cholinergic neuromodulation of inhibitory interneurons facilitates functional integration in whole-brain models' has been provisionally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology.

Before your manuscript can be formally accepted you will need to complete some formatting changes, which you will receive in a follow up email. A member of our team will be in touch with a set of requests.

Please note that your manuscript will not be scheduled for publication until you have made the required changes, so a swift response is appreciated.

IMPORTANT: The editorial review process is now complete. PLOS will only permit corrections to spelling, formatting or significant scientific errors from this point onwards. Requests for major changes, or any which affect the scientific understanding of your work, will cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us now if you or your institution is planning to press release the article. All press must be co-ordinated with PLOS.

Thank you again for supporting Open Access publishing; we are looking forward to publishing your work in PLOS Computational Biology. 

Best regards,

Daniele Marinazzo

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

Daniele Marinazzo

Deputy Editor

PLOS Computational Biology

***********************************************************

PLoS Comput Biol. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008737.r006

Acceptance letter

Daniele Marinazzo

11 Feb 2021

PCOMPBIOL-D-20-01747R2

Cholinergic neuromodulation of inhibitory interneurons facilitates functional integration in whole-brain models

Dear Dr Orio,

I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been formally accepted for publication in PLOS Computational Biology. Your manuscript is now with our production department and you will be notified of the publication date in due course.

The corresponding author will soon be receiving a typeset proof for review, to ensure errors have not been introduced during production. Please review the PDF proof of your manuscript carefully, as this is the last chance to correct any errors. Please note that major changes, or those which affect the scientific understanding of the work, will likely cause delays to the publication date of your manuscript.

Soon after your final files are uploaded, unless you have opted out, the early version of your manuscript will be published online. The date of the early version will be your article's publication date. The final article will be published to the same URL, and all versions of the paper will be accessible to readers.

Thank you again for supporting PLOS Computational Biology and open-access publishing. We are looking forward to publishing your work!

With kind regards,

Alice Ellingham

PLOS Computational Biology | Carlyle House, Carlyle Road, Cambridge CB4 3DN | United Kingdom ploscompbiol@plos.org | Phone +44 (0) 1223-442824 | ploscompbiol.org | @PLOSCompBiol

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Alternative measures of network segregation and integration in the (α, β) parameter space.

    A) Mean participation coefficient PCw (integration) and transitivity Tw (segregation). B-C) Transitions in the α and β axes. Dashed lines represent critical transitions.

    (PDF)

    S2 Fig. Network features in the (α, C1) parameter space.

    A) Global efficiency Ew (integration) and modularity Qw (segregation) of the graphs derived from the sFCs of the BOLD-like signals. B) Transitions in the α axis, for a fixed C1 = 0. C) Transitions in the C1 axis, for a fixed α = 0.5. Dashed lines represent critical transitions.

    (PDF)

    S3 Fig. Alternative measures of network segregation and integration in the (α, r0) parameter space for β = 0.4.

    A-B) Mean participation coefficient PCw (integration) and transitivity Tw (segregation) with A) β = 0 and B) β = 0.4. C-D) Transitions in the α and r0 axes. Dashed lines represent critical transitions.

    (PDF)

    S4 Fig. Simultaneous effect of α, β and r0 in network features.

    A) Global efficiency Ew (integration) and modularity Qw (segregation) of the graphs derived from the sFCs of the BOLD-like signals. B) Transitions in the α axis, for a fixed r0 = 0.5 mV−1. C) Transitions in the r0 axis, for a fixed α = 0.5. Dashed lines represent critical transitions. Both coupling parameters change in parallel following the relationship β = 0.5α.

    (PDF)

    S5 Fig. Effect of the input standard deviation, σ, and mean, μ, in synchronization and integration.

    A-B) Average phase synchrony R¯ measured over the EEG-like signals, and global efficiency Ew computed over the sFC matrices build using the BOLD-like signals. σ and μ reduces the increment of R¯ and Ew mediated by α and β. Both coupling parameters change in parallel following the relationship β = 0.5α. C-D) Phase synchrony R¯ and global efficiency Ew as a function of α and β, for different σ (left hand) and μ (right hand) values.

    (PDF)

    S6 Fig. Network integration computed from mixed BOLD-like signals.

    A) Global efficiency Ew computed in the entire (α, β) parameter space. BOLD-like signals were computed using only the firing rates of pyramidal neurons. B) Ew calculated starting from a summation of the BOLD-like signals simulated using the firing rates of the three neural masses: pyramidal neurons, excitatory and inhibitory interneurons. C) Difference in the global efficiency ΔEw between the two matrices in the (α, β) parameter space. Green values correspond to near-zero difference between the matrices. There is not a noticeable difference between them.

    (PDF)

    S7 Fig. Bifurcation analysis of the modified Jansen & Rit model and comparison to the original model.

    Bifurcations of a single-node model with respect to the mean external input parameter p, at three values of β. Note that β = 0 corresponds to the original Jansen & Rit model. The value depicted in the y-axis is the variable x0. Red and black lines denote stable and unstable fixed points, respectively. Green solid lines and blue dashed lines represent stable and unstable periodic attractors, respectively (denoting the maximum and minimum values of the oscillation). At the right, sample time courses (3 seconds) of the EEG-like signal (C2 x1(t) − C4 x2(t)) at three values of p, and their corresponding power spectra below.

    (PDF)

    S8 Fig. Effect of neuromodulation on the mean oscillatory frequency ω.

    The frequency falls in the Theta range (4-8 Hz) of the EEG spectrum in both the A (α, β) and B (α, r0) parameter spaces. In the first case we fixed r0 = 0.56 mV−1, and in the second one β = 0.4. The frequency drop off matches with phase synchronization (see Fig 5).

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: ReviewersResponse.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: ResponseTo Reviewers2.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files. The code we use to obtain our results is open source and available at https://github.com/vandal-uv/Neuromod2020.


    Articles from PLoS Computational Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES