Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2021 Jul 15;118(29):e2020327118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2020327118

A unified energy-optimality criterion predicts human navigation paths and speeds

Geoffrey L Brown a,b,1, Nidhi Seethapathi a,c,1, Manoj Srinivasan a,d,2
PMCID: PMC8307777  PMID: 34266945

Significance

Why do humans move the way they do? Here, we obtain a physiologically based theory of the speeds and paths with which humans navigate their environment. We measure the metabolic energy cost of walking with turning and show that minimizing this cost explains diverse phenomena involving navigating around obstacles, walking in complex paths, and turning. We explain why humans slow down while turning, avoid sharp turns, do not always use the shortest path, and other naturalistic locomotor phenomena.

Keywords: optimization, locomotion, human movement, predictive theory, metabolic energy

Abstract

Navigating our physical environment requires changing directions and turning. Despite its ecological importance, we do not have a unified theoretical account of non-straight-line human movement. Here, we present a unified optimality criterion that predicts disparate non-straight-line walking phenomena, with straight-line walking as a special case. We first characterized the metabolic cost of turning, deriving the cost landscape as a function of turning radius and rate. We then generalized this cost landscape to arbitrarily complex trajectories, allowing the velocity direction to deviate from body orientation (holonomic walking). We used this generalized optimality criterion to mathematically predict movement patterns in multiple contexts of varying complexity: walking on prescribed paths, turning in place, navigating an angled corridor, navigating freely with end-point constraints, walking through doors, and navigating around obstacles. In these tasks, humans moved at speeds and paths predicted by our optimality criterion, slowing down to turn and never using sharp turns. We show that the shortest path between two points is, counterintuitively, often not energy-optimal, and, indeed, humans do not use the shortest path in such cases. Thus, we have obtained a unified theoretical account that predicts human walking paths and speeds in diverse contexts. Our model focuses on walking in healthy adults; future work could generalize this model to other human populations, other animals, and other locomotor tasks.


Real-world human navigation through our physical environment requires changing direction and turning, maneuvering around obstacles, and moving along complex paths. In one previous study that tracked indoor locomotion over many days, 35 to 45% of steps required turns (1). Despite the importance of turning in ecological settings, we do not have a coherent theoretical account of the paths and speeds observed in such locomotion. Human-subject experiments (27) and mathematical models (5, 815) have suggested that energy optimality explains many aspects of straight-line locomotion, at least approximately. However, we do not know if such energy optimality generalizes to walking while navigating a more complex environment. Here, we obtain a better understanding of locomotion with turning, first quantifying its increased energetic demands and then showing that accounting for these increased energetic demands correctly predicts human navigation paths and speeds in a variety of naturalistic locomotor contexts.

Over the years, researchers have measured human locomotion in a few contexts requiring changing direction—for instance, navigating through angled corridors (16), moving from point to point while having to turn (17), walking through doors, and avoiding obstacles (18, 19). However, previous models aimed at explaining such data used minimization principles that were not physiologically based, required fitting model parameters to behavioral data, were generally fit to only one experiment, and did not generalize to multiple experiments (1719). As we argue later on, these models could not simultaneously explain the paths and speeds observed in curvilinear walking, often predicting zero speeds for simple tasks. Here, we provide a theoretical account that does not have these limitations and is broadly predictive.

We perform human-subject experiments to quantify the metabolic cost of humans walking with turning, measuring how the metabolic cost increases with walking speed and the turning rate. We generalize this empirically derived metabolic-cost landscape to walking along arbitrary paths and then use an optimization-based framework to make a number of behavioral predictions about humans walking in tasks of different complexity. We compared our predictions to five different experiments, each containing a qualitatively different walking or turning task. These five experiments consisted of two behavioral experiments that we performed here and data from three prior studies. Specifically, we predict that humans would walk slower when turning in smaller circles, which we compare with our own behavioral experiments, correctly predicting the lowered walking speeds. We show that the speed at which humans turn in place is approximately predicted by minimizing the cost of turning, again comparing with our own experiments. Finally, minimizing the same metabolic model, we predict more complex walking behavior involving navigation observed in three previous studies: walking freely from point to point (17), walking through doors and avoiding obstacles (18, 19), and walking and turning along an angled corridor (16). We show that energy optimality explains many qualitative and quantitative features of human walking, including not taking sharp turns, path shapes adopted while walking with turning, and speed reductions during turns, as observed in these prior experimental studies (1619), but heretofore not predicted by a single theoretical model. Both our metabolic models and behavioral predictions focus on walking in healthy adults, and we outline the benefits of generalizing our approach to other populations and other locomotor tasks.

Results

Turning Increases Metabolic Cost Substantially.

We measured the metabolic energy expenditure of 17 human subjects while walking with turning. To measure the cost of turning, we instructed the subjects to walk in circles of different radii and at different tangential speeds along the circle. We provided feedback to them to ensure that they were able to perform the required task (Fig. 1A and Materials and Methods). Each subject performed at least 16 trials of four radii and at least four speeds. We measured metabolic energy expenditure using indirect calorimetry—that is, by tracking respiratory oxygen and carbon dioxide flux (20). Fig. 1B shows the resulting mass-normalized metabolic rate—that is, metabolic energy per unit time per unit subject mass Ė—as a function of prescribed speed v and path radius R. These measurements show that for a given prescribed walking speed, the metabolic energy expenditure was higher for smaller radii or, equivalently, for higher path curvature (Fig. 1). For instance, directly comparing the measured metabolic rates at equal prescribed speeds, but different radii, we find that walking at a 1-m radius was more expensive than walking at a 4-m radius by 0.59 W/kg on average (p<4×104 using a right-tailed t test), a 1-m radius was more expensive than a 2-m radius by 0.46 W/kg on average (p=9×104), and a 2-m radius was more expensive than a 4-m radius by 0.14 W/kg on average (p<106). These differences constitute large energy penalties relative to the resting metabolic rate: 40%, 20%, and 10%, respectively, corresponding to Cohen’s d effect sizes of 0.64, 0.62, and 0.15. Because these differences are computed at matched speeds (albeit prescribed speeds), the differences are significant for both metabolic cost per unit time and per unit distance around the circle.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Energy-cost landscape of turning from humans walking in circles. (A) We estimated the metabolic rate as a function of walking speed and turning radius by having subjects walk in circles of a few different radii and at a few different speeds at each radius. Speeds were constrained by having them complete laps at prescribed durations. Metabolic rate was estimated by using respiratory-gas analysis. (B) The metabolic-rate data per unit body mass (Ė) is higher for higher speeds and lower radii. The prescribed speeds and radii (v,R) at which the data were collected are shown as blue dots on the horizontal plane (Ė=0 plane); raw metabolic data points from four representative subjects are shown as green dots (see Dataset S1 for all data). The wireframe surface shows the best-fit model Ė=α0+α1v2+α2ω2, capturing the nonlinear increase with both linear velocity v and angular velocity ω; the model captures 88.1% of the data variance. The four corners of the cost surface have been connected to the horizontal plane to enhance the 3D visualization; a contour plot version of this surface is in SI Appendix, Fig. S1.

Metabolic Rate Increases Nonlinearly with Linear and Angular Speeds.

The total metabolic rate was described well by the following quadratic function of the linear speed v (tangent to the walking path) and the angular speed ω=v/R as follows (Fig. 1B):

Ė=α0+α1v2+α2v2R2=α0+α1v2+α2ω2, [1]

with α0=2.204±0.079 W/kg, α1=1.213±0.054 W/kg/(ms1)2, and α2=0.966±0.061 W/kg/(rads1)2, giving the metabolic rate Ė in W/kg (normalized by body mass), where v is in ms1, R is in meters, and ω is in rads1. The p values for the three coefficients obeyed p<1030, compared to a null constant model of zero coefficients. Eq. 1 explains 88.1% of the empirical metabolic-rate variance over all subjects (this percentage is the statistical R-squared value, the coefficient of determination, not to be confused with radius-squared).

The form for the metabolic-rate expression in Eq. 1 was chosen in analogy to classic work on straight-line walking (2, 21), which found that the metabolic rate is close to linear in v2—that is, Ėα0+α1v2. In a post hoc analysis, we compared the 88.1% variance explained by simple quadratic expression in Eq. 1 to more general quadratic expressions for Ė (with additional v,|ω| and v|ω| terms); such more general quadratic expressions increased the explained variance by less than 0.7% (SI Appendix, Table S1). We used the absolute value |ω|, as we did not distinguish between leftward and rightward turns, ignoring any left–right gait asymmetries. Among such more general quadratic models, the model in Eq. 1 has the lowest Bayesian Information Criterion, a common model-selection criterion that promotes model parsimony while maintaining a good fit to data (22). Similarly, allowing exponents other than two in the model expression, considering Ėα0+α1vγ+α2ωδ, improves the explained variance by less than 0.4% while adding two more model parameters, and so we did not consider it further.

Straight-Line Walking Is a Less Expensive Special Case.

Setting angular speed ω to zero or radius R to infinity in Eq. 1 gives Ė for straight-line walking: α0+α1v2. Thus, as noted, the quadratic expression (Eq. 1) generalizes the classic quadratic expression for the straight-line-walking metabolic rate, namely, α0+α1v2 (2). Previous studies of overground or treadmill straight-line walking (2, 4) have estimated α022.5 W/kg and α10.91.4 W/(kgms1), and our estimates are squarely in this same range. Because the coefficient α2>0 with p<104, the model (Eq. 1) confirms that the estimated metabolic rate is higher for lower radii R for a given tangential speed v. This radius dependence implies, for instance, that at speed v=1.5 m/s, reducing the radius R from infinity to 1 m induces an additional cost (α2v2/R2) of about 43% of the total straight-line-walking metabolic rate (α0+α1v2). This turning cost is about 60% of the net straight-line-walking metabolic rate (α0+α1v2Ėrest)—that is, over and above the resting metabolic rate Ėrest.

As a brief aside, we note that the coefficient α0 has the interpretation of the metabolic rate of walking steadily at very low speeds (v0), which is substantially higher than, and should not be conflated with, the resting metabolic rate Ėrest (here, around 1.4 W/kg). This difference between α0 and Ėrest is similar to differences found by previous studies between the cost of standing still and very slow walking with stepping (2).

An Outline: Energy-Optimal Behavioral Predictions vs. Experiment.

In the rest of this section, we alternate between making behavioral predictions from energy optimization and comparing these predictions with experiment. First, we consider simple tasks such as walking in circles, turning in place, and walking in straight lines: tasks in which the movement path is fixed. For these tasks, we directly use the metabolic model in Eq. 1. Then, we consider more complex tasks that require the movement paths to be selected by the subject: for these tasks, we first generalize the metabolic energy model in Eq. 1 by incorporating other prior metabolic data (as in Eq. 2, presented later), thereby accommodating more general locomotion and arbitrary paths.

Prediction: Optimal Speeds Are Lower for Smaller Circles.

When walking a long-enough distance in a straight line in the absence of time constraints, humans usually walk close to the speed that minimizes the total metabolic cost per unit distance E=Ė/v (2, 4, 5, 23). This energy-optimal speed is sometimes called the maximum range speed (23), as it also maximizes the straight-line distance traveled with a fixed energy budget. Analogously, for walking in circles (Fig. 2A), we hypothesize that humans will use speeds that minimize the total cost per unit distance E=Ė/v=α0/v+(α1+α2/R2)v. This cost per unit distance is minimized when the slope E/v=0—that is, at the speed vopt=α0/(α1+α2/R2) (Fig. 2B). This optimal speed vopt is lower for lower radius R, thus predicting that humans would prefer to walk slower in smaller circles (Fig. 2B). Fig. 2 C and D provide intuition for how the turning cost lowers the optimal speed for circle-walking. The quantity minimized here, namely, the mass-normalized cost per unit distance E, is a scaled version of the cost of transport (10, 23), a nondimensional quantity given by E/g.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Prediction vs. behavior: preferred walking in circles. (A) To test behavioral predictions of energy-optimal walking, we asked subjects to walk on circles of different radii at whatever speeds they found natural. (B) Human-preferred walking speeds and model-predicted optimal walking speeds. Humans walk slower for smaller radii, as also predicted by energy optimality. The yellow box plot shows human preferred walking speed along with individual data points (box indicates 25th percentile, median, and 75th percentile, and whiskers indicate the range). The solid dark blue line is the optimal tangential speed vopt for every radius. Also shown are two bands denoting speeds, for which the metabolic cost per distance is within 1% (lighter blue) and 2% (darker blue) of the optimum cost. Most humans seem to be within 2% of their energy optima. Sensitivity of these predictions to uncertainty in the metabolic model coefficients is shown in SI Appendix, Fig. S2. (C) Metabolic cost per unit distance per unit mass. Minimizing this function at each radius produces model predictions in B, identical to the dark blue line in C. (D) The turning cost per unit distance is linear in velocity (α3v/R) and modifies the walking cost in a manner that the optimal speed is lower for smaller radii or higher curvatures. (E) The optimal metabolic cost per unit distance as a function of the radius.

Experiment: Human Preferred Speeds Are Lower in Smaller Circles, as Predicted by Energy Optimality.

We asked people to walk naturally on circular paths of four different radii and in a straight line (Fig. 2A). As predicted by energy optimality, we found that humans preferred lower speeds for smaller circles (Fig. 2B; ref. 24). The median preferred speed across subjects was well predicted by the energy-optimal speed at every radius, and essentially all of the preferred speeds at any radius were within 2% of optimal energy cost (Fig. 2B). In this walking-in-circles experiment, humans were clearly able to walk at faster or slower speeds than optimal at each radius, as demonstrated in our earlier metabolic trials (Fig. 1). For the special case of straight-line walking, the optimal speed is given by vopt=α0/α1=1.35 m/s, which agrees with typical human preferred walking speeds in previous studies (5, 23, 25), as well as the trials here. Further, for every single subject and every single trial, the data were such that their average preferred speed for a larger-radius circle was higher than that of a smaller circle (vpref,4m>vpref,3m>vpref,2m>vpref,1m).

Corollary: A Straight-Line Path Is Optimal If There Are No Other Constraints or Obstacles.

Conventional wisdom dictates that (in the absence of other constraints), a straight-line path would be energy-optimal to travel a given distance between two points A and B. This conventional wisdom relies on implicit assumptions about the metabolic energy landscape unavailable before our measurements. Assume that the distance to be traveled is long enough (5) so that we minimize the metabolic cost per unit distance E=Ė/v=α0/v+(α1+α2/R2)v, ignoring any small initial or final transient costs. Then, at any speed, this cost is minimized when the turning radius R goes to infinity. This result is reflected in Fig. 2E, which shows the minimum cost per unit distance at any given turning radius, and the minimum is achieved when radius R goes to infinity. Because the straight line both minimizes the distance traveled and the cost per unit distance, the straight line minimizes the total cost for a given distance. Such optimality of straight-line walking will be true for any metabolic-rate model that has positive cost penalty for turning, not necessarily the specific functional form we have considered here. However, as noted earlier, we considered functional forms for the metabolic cost that allowed turning to result in energy reduction—e.g., negative α2 or a linear term in |ω| with a negative coefficient. Such metabolic-cost functions were not justified by our metabolic data (SI Appendix, Table S1). Of course, the optimality of the straight-line path is not generally true in the presence of obstacles or constraints such as those on initial and final body orientation, as consider later in this section.

Prediction: Minimizing Energy Cost of Turning in Place Predicts an Optimal Turning Rate.

Humans often need to turn in place while standing, to reorient their body—to face a new direction. Turning in place or spinning in place (Fig. 3A) is a special case of walking in circles with radius R0 and speed v0, while the angular velocity ω=v/R remains nonzero. Extrapolating using these limits, we obtain the metabolic rate of turning in place to be: Ė=α0+α2ω2. The metabolic cost of turning in place per unit angle (analogous to metabolic cost per unit distance) is Ė/ω=α0/ω+α2ω. This cost per unit angle is optimized by steady optimal turning speed ωopt=α0/α2 = 1.46 rad/s = 83.6 degrees/s (Fig. 3 B and C).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Prediction vs. behavior: turning in place. (A) To further test behavioral predictions of energy optimality, we asked subjects to turn by a given angle α, starting and ending at rest. (B) Metabolic rate and the cost per unit turning angle, obtained by extrapolating the model to turning in place. The blue lines and bands shown denote optimal turning speeds and the set of speeds within 1% or 5% of optimal energy cost. (C) Human preferred turning speeds (yellow box plot and individual data points) largely overlap with the turning speeds within 5% of the optimal cost. Sensitivity of these predictions to uncertainty in the metabolic model coefficients is shown in SI Appendix, Fig. S2.

Experiment: Humans Turn in Place at Close to the Energy-Optimal Turning Rate.

We performed behavioral experiments in which humans turned in place by a fixed turn angle α (Fig. 3A), starting and ending at rest. The average human turning speeds for large turns of 270° and 360° largely overlapped with each other and almost entirely overlapped with the set of steady turning speeds that were within 5% of the optimal turning cost (Fig. 3C).

Two Ways of Generalizing to Complex Paths: Face the Movement Direction or Not.

We now generalize the metabolic cost of walking in circles to walking on arbitrary paths, but, first, we discuss what assumptions such generalizations make. When walking, we can conceptually distinguish between the direction in which our body moves (velocity direction, angle β; Fig. 4A) and the direction in which the body faces (body torso orientation, angle θ). For instance, in normal straight-line walking, these two directions are aligned (θ=β), but in “sideways walking” (6), the body moves perpendicular to how the body faces (Fig. 4B). Thus, it is not essential that we walk in a manner that we always face the movement direction. So, we consider two ways of walking: walking while not always facing the movement direction (Fig. 4C) and walking while always facing the movement direction (Fig. 4D). We call these kinds of walking “holonomic” and “nonholonomic ,” respectively, borrowing this terminology from classical mechanics, control theory, and other prior work on locomotor paths (17, 26, 27). The term nonholonomic simply implies that the system obeys a velocity constraint—here, the constraint is that the body velocity direction is always along body orientation. Holonomic walking has no such velocity constraint, and, thus, nonholonomic walking is a special case of holonomic walking. We generalize the metabolic-cost model of Eq. 1 to both these types of walking (17). Specifically, for holonomic walking, in which the body need not face the movement direction, we use the following revised metabolic cost of the form:

Ė=α0+α1vb2+α2ω2+αsvs2, [2]

where vb is the body velocity component in the forward or anteroposterior direction (in the direction that the body is facing) and vs is the body velocity component in the sideways or medio-lateral direction (perpendicular to how the body is facing) (Fig. 4A). Here, we refer to Eq. 2 along with an additive cost for changing speeds (5) as the “generalized metabolic-cost model.” See Materials and Methods for further details. The rest of this article uses this generalized cost model to make predictions about how people walk in more complex situations involving turning. Because holonomic walking is more general, we use this type of walking to make predictions in the rest of this main manuscript, but we also show results from nonholonomic walking in SI Appendix, Fig. S3.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Two kinds of walking: face where you are going or not. (A) Visual representation of the body from a top view, introducing different notations for the direction in which the body center of mass (CoM) is moving (red arrow) and the body orientation (blue arrow). The components of the center-of-mass velocity are shown to be vf and vs in the anteroposterior (forward) and medio-lateral (sideways) directions, respectively. (B) While forward walking has body orientation aligned with movement direction, it is possible to walk sideways, so that the body orientation is perpendicular to the movement direction. (C) Walking in a manner that the body orientation need not be aligned with the movement direction (holonomic)—that is, the blue and red arrows need not be aligned. (D) Walking in a manner that the body orientation is the same as the movement direction (nonholonomic)—that is, the blue and red arrows are aligned.

Prediction vs. Experiment: Navigating from A to B with Constraints on Initial and Final Direction.

Mombaur et al. (17) performed human-subject trials in which the human started from rest at point A and ended at rest at point B, starting and ending with different body orientations (Fig. 5A). The required body orientations were provided as arrows drawn on the ground. Subjects were not constrained in any other way, say, by obstacles or time limits. Having different required body orientations at A and B required the subjects to turn. For seven different end-point and body-orientation combinations, we computed the metabolically optimal turning trajectory with our model and using trajectory optimization (Materials and Methods and SI Appendix). We performed two versions of the calculation, one holonomic and the other nonholonomic. Remarkably, we found that the holonomic model—that is, allowing body orientation to be different from movement direction—predicted the time progression of body position (x vs. t and y vs. t) and body orientation (θ vs. t) without fitting to these behavioral data (Fig. 5 BD). In both the predicted optimal paths and the human paths, the body orientation was not always aligned with movement direction. Constraining body orientation to be aligned with movement (nonholonomic walking) produced worse predictions of body position and orientation (SI Appendix, Fig. S3; see also ref. 17).

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Prediction vs. behavior: path planning, starting, and ending at rest. (A) Mombaur et al. (17) asked subjects to walk short distances, starting at rest at point A and ending at rest at point B. The subjects had to start facing one direction (light green arrow) and end facing possibly another direction (orange arrow). (BD) The body position (x,y) and body orientation θ as a function of time. Holonomic model predictions are solid dark blue, with a light blue band indicating trajectories withing 5% of the optimum cost; experimental data are dashed dark green, and the best-fit model in Mombaur et al. (17) is indicated as a dashed red line. We see that our energy-optimization-based model predictions mostly pass through the center of the experimental data. Just for targets 3 and 7, subjects started and ended with slightly different body orientations than prescribed, so these were used in the optimizations presented. See SI Appendix, Figs. S1 and S2 for variants of this figure with alternate assumptions. Task 4 involved the most striking difference between these holonomic model predictions vs. the nonholonomic model predictions (SI Appendix, Fig. S1); this task required simply moving sideways by a short distance.

Prediction vs. Experiment: Navigation around and between Two Doorways.

In another set of previous studies (18, 26, 28), researchers instructed human subjects to walk through two sets of doors, A and B, facing in different directions and separated by a few meters (Fig. 6A). The subjects started from rest 2 m before A and ended 2 m beyond B. As in ref. 17, humans chose smooth paths that gradually turned, rather than, say, achieving the same task using sharp turns or too many direction changes (Fig. 6 B and C). Again, we used trajectory optimization to compute the energy-optimal way of performing this task. The resulting optimal trajectories are similar to the human trajectories in data (Fig. 6), which are within 2% of the optimal cost (SI Appendix, Fig. S3). The predictions from the holonomic and nonholonomic models are almost the same, with the nonholonomic model taking a slightly wider turn near the door. For these longer-distance bouts (compared to those in Fig. 5), even when the walker is not constrained to be nonholonomic, it is energy-optimal to be nearly nonholonomic—that is, walk in a manner that the body nearly faces movement direction, as also observed in experiment (26). SI Appendix, Fig. S6 shows how the body movement direction β closely follows body orientation θ. The difference between the two angles (βθ) reduces as the distance traveled increases.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

Prediction vs. behavior: navigating around and through doors. (A) Humans were asked (19) to walk through a doorway at point A (pink parallel lines) to another doorway at point B, stopping 2 m beyond the second door. The second door B had five different locations, as shown (red circles). The walls of the doorways serve as obstacles to be avoided. (B) Human data redrawn from ref. 19 are for head paths. (C) A human or the model is capable of sharp turns and otherwise complex paths to achieve the task. But model predictions for the body path from energy optimality are qualitatively similar to human paths, despite not having to constrain the average velocity as in ref. 19. See SI Appendix, Fig. S5 for bands containing trajectories within 2% of the optimal cost.

An important constraint for the optimal path calculation here, in contrast to those for Fig. 5, is that the body path does not intersect with the doors and has a minimum clearance from the doors. The clearance used is consistent with typical human dimensions (29) and also with behavioral data (16). In the absence of such a clearance constraint, the optimal path ignores the walls of the doors and shows a sharper turn near the end-point B. Thus, explaining human behavior may require considering constraints such as avoiding obstacles (here, the doorways), in addition to minimizing energy-like cost functions.

Corollary: Shortest Paths Are Not Optimal and Not Used by Humans.

In the previous two tasks (Figs. 5 and 6), our model predicted gradually turning paths as also exhibited by the human subjects. These paths are not the shortest paths between the origin and destination: the shortest paths for the tasks in Fig. 5 are straight lines, whereas those for the tasks in Fig. 6 are straight-line paths interspersed by a circular arc around a door. Thus, humans walk for a longer distance than necessary to save energy, even on flat terrain. This nonoptimality of the shortest path is due to the additional cost for turning, without which the shortest path would be energy-optimal.

Prediction vs. Experiment: Humans Slow Down Turning a Corner while Navigating an Angled Corridor.

A common everyday task is turning a corner in an angled corridor (Fig. 7A). A previous study by Dias et al. (16) had subjects walk around angled corridors and measured the walking speeds during the turn. Again, using trajectory optimization, we computed the metabolically optimal path in such angled corridors, specifically computing the walking speed during the turn. We found that the optimal walking speed was lower during the turn and that this turning speed was lower for greater turn angles (Fig. 7 B and C). Further, the experimentally observed human speeds from ref. 16 were almost identical to the model-predicted turning speed; the distribution of human turning speeds overlapped with the model-predicted band of speeds within 2% of the optimal cost (Fig. 7C). Speed reductions during turns were similarly observed by Sreenivasa et al. (30), who considered turning in a cyclical task that alternated between turning and straight-line walking. In all these turning tasks, as predicted by the model, humans do not usually use sharp turns when smooth turns are possible.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7.

Prediction vs. behavior: navigating an angled corridor. (A) A turning task, involving walking along a straight corridor and turning into another corridor angled with respect to the first. (B) Energy-optimal paths for the task. Subject enters the first corridor and leaves the second corridor in a straight-line path at their energy-optimal speed and clears the wall. (C) Experimental data from Dias et al. (16) (mean ± SD) show that the human speeds during the turn are lower for larger turn angles, as also predicted by the model-derived energy-optimal paths. The human speed distribution is entirely contained within the set of speeds within 2% of the optimal cost.

Rectilinear Walking Speeds Are Predicted as a Special Case.

The optimality criterion proposed here predicts rectilinear or straight-line walking phenomena as a special case. Specifically, it predicts that typical steady human walking speeds should be around vopt=α0/α1=1.35 m/s, which agrees with preferred walking speeds over longer distances in previous studies (5, 23, 25). Seethapathi and Srinivasan (5) used a straight-line walking metabolic model along with a cost for changing speeds to predict lowered walking speeds for short-distance bouts. Our generalized metabolic-cost model contains the model of ref. 5 as a special case (by construction) and, thus, also predicts that humans should walk systematically slower for shorter distances. Similarly, Handford and Srinivasan (6) showed that when asked to walk sideways, humans walk close to the energy-optimal sideways walking speed (about 0.6 m/s). Again, the generalized holonomic model contains the sideways walking model of ref. 6 as a special case (by construction) and makes similarly low-speed predictions for sideways walking.

Prediction vs. Experiment: To Go Sideways, Walk Sideways or Turn and Walk Forward.

Humans do not usually walk sideways. Handford and Srinivasan (6) showed that sideways walking costs three times more energy than walking forward at their respective optimal speeds. Now, consider a situation in which someone wants to go from A to B, but is initially (and finally) facing perpendicular to the line AB (SI Appendix, Fig. S7). That is, they want to move sideways—e.g., they are working at a kitchen counter and want to move sideways. Should they walk sideways or turn by 90° and walk facing forward? To make a prediction, we compared the cost of walking sideways and the cost of turning and walking forward and turning again. This comparison predicted that humans should walk sideways for distances less than 0.8 m; for larger distances, walking sideways is more expensive than turning and walking facing forward. Indeed, target 4 in Fig. 5 asks subjects to travel sideways by 0.4 m, starting and ending facing forward (17). Subjects, as predicted, did not turn and walk forward; instead, they just stepped sideways while mostly facing forward.

The Predictive Power of Minimizing Metabolic Energy vs. Other Optimality Hypotheses.

We have predicted a wide variety of experimentally measured human locomotor behavior, with no fitting to these behavioral data, by directly minimizing an empirically based metabolic model of walking. We now briefly compare the energy-cost-based hypothesis with some other hypotheses that have been explored. Smooth body trajectories can be predicted by cost functions that maximize smoothness, such as acceleration, “jerk” (derivative of acceleration), or “snap” (second derivative of acceleration). Such cost functions have been successful in arm reaching (31), but they have also been employed to predict walking trajectories (17, 18). However, such smoothness-maximizing cost functions, taken seriously, produce two unecological predictions. First, they cannot predict the velocity at which people move: specifically, the optimal way to minimize jerk or snap is to perform a task with infinitesimal speed over an arbitrarily long period. See SI Appendix, section S4 for a mathematical account of using these smoothness objectives. Thus, such smoothness-maximizing cost functions require a constraint on the average velocity of the task to produce meaningful results. In contrast, our metabolic-energy approach naturally produces an optimal velocity for any task, without having to constrain it. A second consequence of the jerk-minimization hypothesis, even with a velocity constraint, is that it produces “scale-invariant solutions.” That is, for the tasks in Fig. 6A, it produces paths that “look the same” for a 10-m walk vs. a 10-km walk, producing many-kilometer-long path excursions that humans would never use.

Arechavaleta et al. (19) used an objective function equal to the integral of (v2+κ˙2) over the path, where κ˙ is the rate of change of path curvature. Again, minimizing this objective without a further constraint predicts that humans should move at infinitesimal velocity. So, in their calculations, Arechavaleta et al. (19) constrained the total time. Thus, these minimization hypotheses require further assumptions about human behavior, which need not be made with the more parsimonious energetics-based approach.

Finally, Mombaur et al. (17) used a model-fitting procedure to select an objective function that best predicts human walking trajectories in their experiments. Their objective function had terms related to linear and angular velocity acceleration, work, jerk, and task time duration. However, their best-fit cost function, because it is dominated by linear and angular acceleration terms, cannot explain observed speeds for walking in circles, turning in place, or walking in a straight line for longer distances. Thus, each of these previous cost functions could be considered as overfit to one or two experimental conditions and cannot predict all of the diverse phenomena predicted by our model. On the other hand, the true objective may well contain terms analogous to those used in these prior works (1719), in addition to those in our metabolic model; for instance, smoothness-promoting terms related to acceleration or jerk, as a proxy for costs related to muscle forces and force rates (3234). Such terms may be useful in explaining smoother speed changes (better than our model can) when speed changes are needed, for instance, during gait initiation and termination (35).

Discussion

We have provided a unified theoretical account of human navigation paths and speeds. We first measured the energetics of humans walking with turning and showed that the energy cost has a substantial dependence on path curvature. Turning increased the locomotor cost, and this turning cost increased with the turning speed. We then used the experimentally derived metabolic-energy model to predict energy-optimal walking behavior in various contexts, explaining many disparate experimentally measured and ecological human locomotor behavior, some via additional experiments that we performed and some by comparison with data from prior experimental studies on human navigation. Importantly, our theoretical account also makes predictions about straight-line locomotion as special cases, predicting steady walking speeds and short distance speeds when turning is not required. Thus, we have presented a unified theoretical account of both straight-line and more general curvilinear human walking.

We showed that the tendency to slow down when turning and slow down more when turning in a smaller radius can be explained by energy optimality. One alternate hypothesis for slowing down while turning is to avoid slipping. While fast-moving cars and bicycles slow down on curves to avoid slipping, humans in our experiments were far from any danger of slipping. We estimated the foot–ground friction coefficient as μ=0.6 to 1.2, giving friction cone angles of 30° to 50° (=tan1μ). But the maximum leg angle in our circle walking (1 m at 1.5 m/s) was 12°, much less than the friction cone angles, giving a large safety factor from slipping.

The speed reductions found here due to path curvature are reminiscent of speed reductions when humans try to run as fast as possible around a circle (36, 37). However, these tasks are different in one important respect: while our subjects are able to walk faster or slower compared to their preferred speeds, maximal-speed running does not have this property (by definition). Nevertheless, there may be mechanistic similarities between the two cases. As was initially thought for maximal running (36, 37), we might hypothesize that the increased walking metabolic cost may be due to the necessity for producing a centripetal force; see refs. 33, 34, and 3841 for accounts of force costs in locomotion. However, using elementary formulas for centripetal accelerations (36), we estimated that for the fastest walking speed at the smallest radius, the leg-force requirements increased only by 1.3% (SI Appendix, section S3). This small force increase cannot directly account for the nearly 50% increases in metabolic costs we have measured. Similarly, if we conceptualize the additional centripetal acceleration as increasing the “effective gravity” (resulting in the increased leg forces) by 1 to 2%, then the leg-work requirements would also increase by a similar small percentage, and not by over 50% (12, 42, 43). One might speculate that the larger increases in metabolic rate may be partly due to recruitment of additional muscles that may not be at their optimal operating regimes (37).

The cost of turning could also be due to the work necessary to turn the body orientation, given that the body has a nonzero rotational inertia (44). Per our assumed metabolic-cost-model form, the metabolic cost of turning in place was Ė=α0+α2ω2. Turning in place only requires body-orientation changes and does not require centripetal forces. Thus, this cost model for turning in place is consistent with the premise that most of the increased cost due to turning (namely, α2ω2) is to accomplish body-orientation changes (SI Appendix, section S3). The increased metabolic cost could potentially be understood by measuring the body movements and the ground-reaction forces (24, 45, 46), performing inverse dynamics (15) and examining which joints have greater torques or perform more work.

Our metabolic-cost model is empirical in nature, so that to generalize it to different situations (e.g., running or uneven terrain), we may need to repeat the metabolic experiments for that new situation. An alternate path to potential generalization is via a three-dimensional (3D) dynamical model of a biped (47, 48), first showing that it is able to predict the walking metabolic measurements here (and, along the way, also incorporating the stepping dynamics, ignored here for parsimony). But, such 3D biped models may still not generalize to other human populations—for instance, those with movement disorders, whether due to musculoskeletal or neurological differences, because predictive understanding of movement behavior with movement disorders without task- or population-specific fitting is largely open. On a related note, we have assumed left–right symmetry in fitting the metabolic-cost model (Eq. 1), but bodily asymmetries may translate to energetic asymmetries in turning, e.g., in unilateral amputee populations. Accounting for such asymmetries in the metabolic model, one can then examine whether no turning (ω=0) still minimizes the metabolic rate for speed v and whether a straight-line path remains energy-optimal.

One could conjecture that there are greater stability issues while turning and that these stability issues contribute to the subjects being cautious and lowering their speed. However, such conjecture seems unnecessary, as energy minimization seems to largely explain the speed reductions. An open question in movement control is to what extent humans prioritize energy or effort on the one hand and stability or robustness to uncertainty on the other (49). This question is beyond the scope of this study, as we have based our behavioral predictions on empirically derived energy costs. The experimentally measured energy costs already include any trade-offs that humans had to make to walk efficiently and stably (50, 51). Thus, we should distinguish our empirical optimality criterion from the theoretical limit of true energy optimality, requiring perfect control and the absence of perturbations or uncertainty.

In the real world, there may be other, additional concerns that may make a human walk faster than energy-optimal—for instance, a cost for time or constraints on time taken to complete the movement (4, 52). Here, we have considered conditions where no such explicit time pressure exists. Deviations from deterministic energy optimality may also be due to optimality in a Bayesian or probabilistic sense (53) in the presence of uncertainly or due to interactions with or navigating around another moving human.

It is sometimes argued that energy optimality or energy economy may be useful only in “steady-state tasks,” as opposed to “transient tasks,” or may only be useful when the energy saved is substantial (5). Here, we have shown broad agreement of behavior with empirical energy optimality in short transient tasks (e.g., the turning part of a corner) that consume a very small amount of energy. For instance, we estimate the total energy cost of a turn in the angled corridor (Fig. 7) to be about 10 J/kg. So, the savings relative to a nonoptimal turn (e.g., not slowing down or using a sharper turn) are a small fraction of this cost, equivalent to just over a second of resting-energy expenditure. Here, as in many of the situations we considered, the experimentally observed behavior was within 2% of the optimal energy costs from the model, corresponding to similarly small amounts of energy differences (0.2 J/kg, equivalent to resting-energy expenditure for one-seventh of a second). That energy optimality provides an account of diverse transient behavior with low energy requirements is perhaps an indication of how much the nervous system values energy savings, all else being equal. Our results are agnostic to how the near-energy optimality is achieved, whether it is hard-wired evolutionarily, acquired while learning to walk during childhood, or the energy-optimal trajectories are computed in real time by the nervous system (54, 55). It is likely a mixture of all such mechanisms: energy optimality correctly predicted lowered walking speeds for shorter distances [a task with low total energy (5)], walking speeds in sideways walking [an uncommon task (6)], and stride frequency in the presence of an external exoskeleton [an uncommon task with dynamic changes in the energy landscape (7, 54)].

The neural mechanisms underlying such energy optimality or locomotor navigation are yet to be elucidated (56). Further, the aspects of navigation we focused on were path and speed selection, when all information about the world is fully available, and we did not consider aspects involving sensing, information gathering, and course correction, or topological aspects in which one among multiple (potentially locally optimal) paths around obstacles need to be selected (57, 58).

Our results suggest further behavioral experiments to test model predictions and inform improvements to the model: for instance, walking through via points with freedom to choose the intervening path, comparing walking sideways vs. turning, walking around obstacles, walking around moving obstacles (such as other people), etc. We obtained a cost of spinning in place by extrapolation, whose accuracy can be improved by using smaller radii. Because of this extrapolation, we would a priori expect the current model to be less reliable in tasks that require sharp direction changes. Mechanisms for the turning cost could be probed by predicting and testing experimentally how the cost varies after various manipulations of the system: adding mass or moment of inertia to the trunk or the legs (44) or providing centripetal forces via a tether, as in ref. 37. Our empirical metabolic-cost model could be tested further by energetic measurements of humans walking on sinusoidal paths, achieved by moving side to side on treadmills. Such curvilinear paths with nonconstant curvature may allow us to disambiguate energy-cost dependence on v and ω vs. their derivatives, which may be more significant for more unsteady tasks.

The metabolic model for curvilinear locomotion presented here may help improve the estimates of daily metabolic expenditure obtained by, say, using wearable devices. Studies have found that about 20% of steps in household settings (59) and 35 to 45% of steps in common walking tasks in home and office environments involve turns (1), so neglecting the cost of turning may result in erroneous estimates of energy expenditure. Further ambulatory studies tracking people over many days with wearable sensors (60) could help to estimate the magnitude of this error.

As noted earlier, reduced maximal running speeds around a circle have been measured (36, 37), but submaximal running studies measuring metabolic costs have not been performed. Generalizing our metabolic model to running may better help estimate the metabolic cost during sports (e.g., soccer), which involve extensive speed and direction changes. Understanding human locomotion while turning would also be a useful tool in rehabilitation or assistive robotics. Robotic legs, prostheses, and assistive devices are often designed with an emphasis on straight-line walking. So, better understanding turning mechanics (for instance, by providing targets of turning performance) may inform designing for real-world scenarios where curvilinear locomotion is essential.

In conclusion, through experiments and mathematical models, we have provided a unified theoretical predictive account of human walking in non-straight-line paths and with turning from the perspective of energy optimality. We have suggested further experiments to test model predictions, to inform model improvements, and to generalize the models to other populations and other tasks.

Materials and Methods

We performed three different experimental studies, one for measuring the metabolic cost of turning and two for measuring human behavior while turning. We performed multiple model-based optimization calculations to predict energy-optimal trajectories and speeds under different task constraints to compare with a number of different behavioral experiments, including our own.

Experiment: Metabolic Cost of Humans Walking in Circles.

All experiments were approved by The Ohio State University’s Institutional Review Board, and all subjects participated with informed consent. Subjects were instructed to walk along circles drawn on the ground (Fig. 1A). The subjects were instructed to keep the circle directly beneath their feet or between their two feet, but not entirely to one side of their feet. All subjects walked with the circle between their feet with nonzero step width. We used four different circle radii (R=1,2,3,4 m, Nradii=4). At each radius, subjects performed four walking trials, each with a different constant tangential speed v in the range 0.8 to 1.58 m/s, resulting in Ntrials=16 trials per subject; one subject performed fewer trials (Ntrials=13). Tangential speeds were prescribed by specifying a duration for each lap around the circle. A timer provided auditory feedback at the end of every half-lap duration (for R=3,4 m) or full-lap duration (for R=1,2 m), so that subjects could speed up or slow down as necessary. Within a few laps of such auditory-feedback-driven training, subjects walked at the desired average speed, completing each lap almost coincident with the desired lap time. Subjects maintained the speed with continued auditory feedback for 6 to 7 min: 4 min for achieving biomechanical and metabolic steady state and 2 to 3 min for computing an average metabolic rate Ė. Subjects used clockwise or counterclockwise circles as preferred. Subjects were instructed to walk and never jog or run; all subjects always walked. Overall, this resulted in nearly 35 h of subjects walking in circles.

The trial order was randomized over speed and radius for seven subjects (mass 77.3±10 kg, height 1.79±0.05 m, mean ± SD, and age range 22 to 27), whose data are presented in detail in Results. For 10 other subjects (mass 73±14 kg, height 1.75±0.13, mean ± SD, and age range 21 to 27), the trial order increased monotonically in speed and radius. The overall regression relations were similar when both sets of data were analyzed in the same manner, suggesting no large order effect. Metabolic rate per unit mass Ė was estimated during resting and circular walking by using respiratory gas analysis (Oxycon Mobile with wind shield, < 1 kg): Ė=16.58V˙O2+4.51V˙CO2W/kg with volume rates V˙ in mLs−1kg−1 (20). Subjects exhibited small, but systematic, differences between prescribed lap times and lap times estimated from video. So, in subsequent analyses, we used corrected values for v and ω by using the estimated lap times in their calculation (speed = circumference/lap time, angular speed = 2π/lap time). See Dataset S1 for the collected data, including these corrections. To improve estimates of the steady-state metabolic rate and to test if the transients had subsided, we fit an exponential to the last 3 min of metabolic data and determined the extrapolated steady state (61, 62). This extrapolation resulted in less than 2% changes in any of the coefficients in Eq. 1, compared to the standard procedure of using the mean metabolic rate over the last 2 or 3 min. This comparison suggests that the standard procedure suffices. Reported significance of metabolic differences across radii were tested via one-sided t tests with a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Linear regressions used fitlm in MATLAB.

Experiment: Preferred Walking Speeds in Circles.

Subjects’ preferred walking speeds were measured by asking them to walk in a straight line and along circles (24) of radii (R) equal to 1 m, 2 m, 3 m, and 4 m at whatever speed they found comfortable (Fig. 2A); the subjects walked for about 100 m in each of these trials (four 4-m laps, eight 2-m laps, etc.). The subjects were told beforehand how many laps they would need to complete and were asked to do whatever felt normal or natural. We measured the time duration T for the second half of the walk from video and estimated the average tangential speed as the distance traveled around the circle divided by the time duration; that is, the average tangential speed was (2πRnlaps)/T, where nlaps is the number of laps considered. Two trials were performed for each radius, and all trials were in random order of radii. The subject population for these trials (mass 73.6 ± 10 kg, height 1.74 ± 0.13 m, age 22.6 ± 1.7 y, and 90 trials with N=9) was distinct from those in the previous experiment.

Experiment: Preferred Turning-in-Place Speeds.

Subjects’ (mass 73.4 ± 11 kg, height 1.75 ± 0.10 m, age 26 ± 5 y, N=10 distinct from earlier samples) preferred turning speeds were measured by asking them to turn in place by 90°, 180°, 270°, and 360° and do whatever felt natural (Fig. 3A). Three trials were performed for each turn angle, and the trial orders were randomized. Subjects were free to turn clockwise or counterclockwise in any trial. The average angular velocity of turn was computed by estimating the time taken for turning from video and using the prescribed turn angle. Our goal was to compare preferred turning speeds with model-based predictions of steady-state turning speeds—that is, speeds not affected by the starting and stopping. We found that the average speeds for 270° and 360° turns were similar (Fig. 3), so we infer that these speeds are close to the steady-state speeds. Thus, we used the turning speeds for 270° and 360° turns to test predictions of steady-state turning speeds.

Model: Metabolic Cost of Arbitrary Walking Paths.

Our walking-in-circles metabolic experiments constrained the circular paths that the feet travel on rather than the paths that the body travels in. Thus, the radius R and the velocity v in Eq. 1 correspond to the mean trajectory of the feet, rather than the center of mass. If the foot travels in a circle of radius R and has an effective tangential velocity v, the body center of mass travels in a circle of smaller radius Rb and slightly lower tangential velocity vb. This is because the body leans into the circle, so that the hip is closer to the center of the circle than the foot (36). We first obtained a body-based description of the empirical metabolic cost: Ė=α0+α1vb2+α2ωb2, where ωb=ω=v/R=vb/Rb for circle walking. We find α0=2.32 W/kg, α1=1.28 W/kg/ (ms1)2, and α2=1.02 W/kg/ (rads1)2. These coefficients explain the metabolic data roughly as well as the original model (explaining about 88% variance).

Any noncircular or non-straight-line walking path can be described as a curve with constantly changing curvature. That is, each point on the curve has a distinct radius of curvature. If we assume nonholonomic walking, in which the body always faces the movement direction, we can directly apply a metabolic rate of the form Ė=α0+α1v2+α2ωb2, where vb is the instantaneous body velocity, ωb=vb/Rb is the body angular velocity, and Rb is the instantaneous radius of curvature. To generalize to holonomic walking—that is, allowing the body to not always face the velocity direction—we distinguished between the body velocity component along the body orientation vf (forward) and the body velocity component perpendicular to the body orientation vs (sideways). We then used a metabolic-cost model of the form: Ė=α0+α1vf2+α2ωb2+αsvs2. This is the same as Eq. 2. Here, the additional term αsvs2 is the incremental cost of sideways walking (6). Handford and Srinivasan (6) estimated the coefficient αs as roughly 7.8 W/kg/(ms1)2.

Because walking along arbitrary paths may involve or require changing walking speeds, we included the additive metabolic cost of changing speeds, characterized by Seethapathi and Srinivasan (5). This study showed that accounting for this cost predicts lower speeds for short-distance walking bouts using energy optimality, as in humans (5, 35). Eq. 2, in addition to this work-based cost for changing speeds, is what we refer to as the generalized metabolic-cost model in this manuscript. See SI Appendix for more details about the metabolic-cost model.

Model: Energy-Optimality-Based Behavioral Predictions.

We compared measured experimental human behavior in a number of different walking tasks to the energy-optimal walking behavior predictions. The energy-optimal walking behavior was obtained by minimizing the total metabolic cost of the walking task. For the simplest two tasks—namely, walking in circle and turning in place (Figs. 2 and 3)—the optimization assumed steady state and required only basic calculus. So, the complete analytical reasoning for the prediction is provided in Results. For more complex walking tasks, where the walking path is not predetermined (Figs. 57), we solved for the total time duration, body position, body orientation, and their derivatives as functions of time using numerical trajectory-optimization methods (12). For this trajectory optimization, we used the metabolic-cost function described in the previous paragraph. We performed two versions of the optimization, holonomic and nonholonomic, with the latter obeying the constraint that the body always faces the velocity direction. We solved additional optimization problems to obtain trajectories within a certain percent of the optimal cost. See SI Appendix for more mathematical details of the numerical optimization.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary File
pnas.2020327118.sd01.txt (19.7KB, txt)
Supplementary File

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NSF Grant CMMI-1254842 and its writing in part by an NIH grant R01GM135923-01. We thank Carmen Swain and Blake Holderman for help regarding metabolic equipment during early pilot testing; Alison Sheets for comments on an early draft; and V. Joshi for help with some experimental setups.

Footnotes

The authors declare no competing interest.

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. A.A.B. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial Board.

This article contains supporting information online at https://www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.2020327118/-/DCSupplemental.

Data Availability

All study data are included in the article and/or SI Appendix.

References

  • 1.Glaister B. C., Bernatz G. C., Klute G. K., Orendurff M. S., Video task analysis of turning during activities of daily living. Gait Posture 25, 289–294 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ralston H. J., Energy-speed relation and optimal speed during level walking. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 17, 277–283 (1958). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Donelan J. M., Kram R., Kuo A. D., Mechanical and metabolic determinants of the preferred step width in human walking. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 268, 1985–1992 (2001). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Long L. L., Srinivasan M., Walking, running, and resting under time, distance, and average speed constraints: Optimality of walk-run-rest mixtures. J. R. Soc. Interface 10, 20120980 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Seethapathi N., Srinivasan M., The metabolic cost of changing walking speeds is significant, implies lower optimal speeds for shorter distances, and increases daily energy estimates. Biol. Lett. 11, 20150486 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Handford M. L., Srinivasan M., Sideways walking: Preferred is slow, slow is optimal, and optimal is expensive. Biol. Lett. 10, 20131006 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Selinger J. C., Wong J. D., Simha S. N., Donelan J. M., How humans initiate energy optimization and converge on their optimal gaits. J. Exp. Biol. 222, jeb198234 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Minetti A. E., Alexander R. M. N., A theory of metabolic costs for bipedal gaits. J. Theor. Biol. 186, 467–476 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kuo A. D., A simple model of bipedal walking predicts the preferred speed–step length relationship. J. Biomech. Eng. 123, 264–269 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Srinivasan M., Ruina A., Computer optimization of a minimal biped model discovers walking and running. Nature 439, 72–75 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ackermann M., Van Den Bogert A. J., Optimality principles for model-based prediction of human gait. J. Biomech. 43, 1055–1060 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Srinivasan M., Fifteen observations on the structure of energy minimizing gaits in many simple biped models. J. R. Soc. Interfac. 8, 74–98 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Miller R. H., Umberger B. R., Hamill J., Caldwell G. E., Evaluation of the minimum energy hypothesis and other potential optimality criteria for human running. Proc. Biol. Sci. 279, 1498–1505 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Joshi V., Srinivasan M., Walking on a moving surface: Energy-optimal walking motions on a shaky bridge and a shaking treadmill can reduce energy costs below normal. Proc. Roy. Soc. A 471, 20140662 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Falisse A., et al. , Rapid predictive simulations with complex musculoskeletal models suggest that diverse healthy and pathological human gaits can emerge from similar control strategies. J. R. Soc. Interface 16, 20190402 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Dias C., Ejtemai O., Sarvi M., Shiwakoti N., Pedestrian walking characteristics through angled corridors: An experimental study. Transport. Res. Rec. 2421, 41–50 (2014). [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Mombaur K., Truong A., Laumond J.-P., From human to humanoid locomotion: An inverse optimal control approach. Aut. Robots 28, 369–383 (2010). [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Pham Q., Hicheur H., Arechavaleta G., Laumond J., Berthoz A., The formation of trajectories during goal-oriented locomotion in humans. II. A maximum smoothness model. Eur. J. Neur. 26, 2391–2403 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Arechavaleta G., Laumond J., Hiceur H., Berthoz A., An optimality principle governing human walking. IEEE Trans. Robot. 24, 5–14 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brockway J. M., Derivation of formulae used to calculate energy expenditure in man. Hum. Nutr. Clin. Nutr. 41C, 463–471 (1987). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bobbert A. C., Energy expenditure in level and grade walking. J. Appl. Physiol. 15, 1015–1021 (1960). [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Neath A. A., Cavanaugh J. E., The Bayesian information criterion: Background, derivation, and applications. Wiley Interdis. Rev. Comput. Stat. 4, 199–203 (2012). [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Srinivasan M., Optimal speeds for walking and running, and walking on a moving walkway. Chaos 19, 026112 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Orendurff M. S., et al. , The kinematics and kinetics of turning: Limb asymmetries associated with walking a circular path. Gait Posture 23, 106–111 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bohannon R. W., Andrews A. W., Normal walking speed: A descriptive meta-analysis. Physiotherapy 97, 182–189 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Arechavaleta G., Laumond J.-P., Hicheur H., Berthoz A., “The nonholonomic nature of human locomotion: A modeling study” in The First IEEE/RAS-EMBS International Conference on Biomedical Robotics and Biomechatronics, 2006. BioRob 2006 (IEEE, Piscataway, NJ, 2006), pp. 158–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Neimark J. I., Fufaev N. A., Dynamics of Nonholonomic Systems (Translations of Mathematical Monographs, American Mathematical Society, Providence, RI, 1972), vol. 33. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hicheur H., Pham Q., Arechavaleta G., Laumond J., Berthoz A., The formation of trajectories during goal-oriented locomotion in humans. i. a stereotyped behaviour. Eur. J. Neur. 26, 2376–2390 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Fruin J. J., Pedestrian Planning and Design (Revised Edition, Elevator World, Inc., Mobile, AL, 1987). [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Sreenivasa M. N., Frissen I., Souman J. L., Ernst M. O., Walking along curved paths of different angles: The relationship between head and trunk turning. Exp. Brain Res. 191, 313–320 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Vivani P., Flash T., Minimum-jerk model, two-thirds power law, and isochrony: Converging approaches to movement planning. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 21, 32–53 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Wong J. D., Cluff T., Kuo A. D., The energetic basis for smooth human arm movements. bioRxiv [Preprint] (2020). 10.1101/2020.12.28.424067 (Accessed 1 February 2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 33.Kram R., Muscular force or work: What determines the metabolic energy cost of running. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 28, 138–143 (2000). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Doke J., Donelan M. J., Kuo A. D., Mechanics and energetics of swinging the human leg. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 439–445 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Miff S. C., Childress D. S., Gard S. A., Meier M. R., Hansen A. H.. Temporal symmetries during gait initiation and termination in nondisabled ambulators and in people with unilateral transtibial limb loss. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 42, 175–182 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Greene P. R., Running on flat turns: Experiments, theory, and applications. J. Biomech. Eng. 107, 96 (1985). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Chang Y.-H., Kram R., Limitations to maximum running speed on flat curves. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 971–982 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Griffin T. M., Roberts T. J., Kram R., Metabolic cost of generating muscular force in human walking: Insights from load-carrying and speed experiments. J. Appl. Physiol. 95, 172–183 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Roberts T. J., Kram R., Weyand P. G., Taylor C. R., Energetics of bipedal running. I. Metabolic cost of generating force. J. Exp. Biol. 201, 2745–2751 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hubel T. Y., Usherwood J. R., Children and adults minimise activated muscle volume by selecting gait parameters that balance gross mechanical power and work demands. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 2830–2839 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Biewener A. A., Farley C. T., Roberts T. J., Temaner M., Muscle mechanical advantage of human walking and running: Implications for energy cost. J. Appl. Physiol. 97, 2266–2274 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Donelan J. M., Kram R., The effect of reduced gravity on the kinematics of human walking: A test of the dynamic similarity hypothesis for locomotion. J. Exp. Biol. 200, 3193–3201 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Grabowski A., Farley C. T., Kram R., Independent metabolic costs of supporting body weight and accelerating body mass during walking. J. Appl. Physiol. 98, 579–583 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Qiao M., Brown B., Jindrich D. L., Compensations for increased rotational inertia during human cutting turns. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 432–443 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Courtine G., Schieppati M., Human walking along a curved path. II. Gait features and EMG patterns. Eur. J. Neur. 18, 191–205 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Turcato A. M., Godi M., Giordano A., Schieppati M., Nardone A., The generation of centripetal force when walking in a circle: Insight from the distribution of ground reaction forces recorded by plantar insoles. J. NeuroEng. Rehabil. 12, 4 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Miller R. H., A comparison of muscle energy models for simulating human walking in three dimensions. J. Biomech. 47, 1373–1381 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Handford M. L., Srinivasan M., Robotic lower limb prosthesis design through simultaneous computer optimizations of human and prosthesis costs. Sci. Rep. 6, 19983 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Saglam C. O., Byl K., “Quantifying the trade-offs between stability versus energy use for underactuated biped walking” in 2014 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems (IEEE, Piscataway, NJ, 2014), pp. 2550–2557. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Seethapathi N., Srinivasan M., Step-to-step variations in human running reveal how humans run without falling. Elife 8, e38371 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Joshi V., Srinivasan M., A controller for walking derived from how humans recover from perturbations. J. R. Soc. Interface 16, 20190027 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Summerside E. M., Kram R., Ahmed A. A., Contributions of metabolic and temporal costs to human gait selection. J. R. Soc. Interface 15, 20180197 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Körding K. P., Wolpert D. M., Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427, 244–247 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Selinger J. C., Connor S. M. O., Wong J. D., Donelan J. M., Humans can continuously optimize energetic cost during walking. Curr. Biol. 25, 2452–2456 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wong J. D., Connor S. M. O., Selinger J. C., Donelan J. M., Contribution of blood oxygen and carbon dioxide sensing to the energetic optimization of human walking. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 1425–1433 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Edvardsen V., Bicanski A., Burgess N., Navigating with grid and place cells in cluttered environments. Hippocampus 30, 220–232 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Warren W. H., Fajen B. R., “Behavioral dynamics of visually guided locomotion” in Coordination: Neural, Behavioral and Social Dynamics, Fuchs A., Jirsa V. K., Eds. (Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, 2008), pp. 45–75 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Baxter B. A., Warren W. H., Route selection in barrier avoidance. Gait & Posture 80, 192–198 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Sedgeman R., Goldie P., Iansek R., “Development of a measure of turning during walking” in Advancing Rehabilitation Conference Proceedings (La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia, 1994), pp. 26–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Daley M. A., Channon A. J., Nolan G. S., Hall J., Preferred gait and walk–run transition speeds in ostriches measured using GPS-IMU sensors. J. Exp. Biol. 219, 3301–3308 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Selinger J. C., Donelan J. M., Estimating instantaneous energetic cost during non-steady-state gait. J. Appl. Physiol. 117, 1406–1415 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Zhang J., et al. , Human-in-the-loop optimization of exoskeleton assistance during walking. Science 356, 1280–1284 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary File
pnas.2020327118.sd01.txt (19.7KB, txt)
Supplementary File

Data Availability Statement

All study data are included in the article and/or SI Appendix.


Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES