Abstract
Laboratory experiments using fear conditioning and extinction protocols help lay the groundwork for designing, testing, and optimizing innovative treatments for anxiety-related disorders. Yet, there is limited basic research on fear conditioning and extinction in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). This is surprising because exposure-based treatments based on associative learning principles are among the most popular and effective treatment options for OCD. Here, we systematically review and critically assess existing aversive conditioning and extinction studies of OCD. Across 12 studies, there was moderate evidence that OCD is associated with abnormal acquisition of conditioned responses that differ from comparison groups. There was relatively stronger evidence of OCD’s association with impaired extinction processes. This included multiple studies finding elevated conditioned responses during extinction learning and poorer threat/safety discrimination during recall, although a minority of studies yielded results inconsistent with this conclusion. Overall, the conditioning model holds value for OCD research, but more work is necessary to clarify emerging patterns of results and increase clinical translational utility to the level seen in other anxiety-related disorders. We detail limitations in the literature and suggest next steps, including modeling OCD with more complex conditioning methodology (e.g., semantic/conceptual generalization, avoidance) and improving individual-differences assessment with dimensional techniques.
Keywords: obsessive-compulsive disorder, fear-conditioning, disgust-conditioning, classical conditioning, acquisition, extinction, exposure therapy, clinical translation
1. Introduction
Translating conditioning and extinction principles and paradigms to effective behavioral treatments has led to the development of effective first-line treatments for anxiety-related disorders, most notably exposure therapies (Foa & McLean, 2016; Rachman, 2015). One of the disorders that benefits from exposure intervention is obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). OCD is an anxiety-related disorder associated with considerable personal and occupational burden (Huppert et al., 2009; Markarian et al., 2010) and characterized by entrenched and intrusive obsessional cognitive content (including thoughts, images, and impulses) and interfering compulsive behaviors. The gold-standard exposure intervention for OCD is exposure and response prevention (EX/RP; Foa et al., 2012; Meyer, 1966; Rachman et al., 1971), which primarily involves techniques that teach the patient to resist enacting a compulsion while enduring obsession-related distress. Although EX/RP is the most effective available behavioral treatment for OCD (Olatunji et al., 2013), a significant minority of patients either do not respond to the treatment, drop-out, or show symptom remission and do not maintain treatment gain (Abramowitz, 1996; Fisher & Wells, 2005; Foa & McLean, 2016; Jacoby & Abramowitz, 2016; Johnco et al., 2020; Simpson et al., 2006). A noteworthy statistic regarding EX/RP is that up to 60% of patients with OCD who complete the treatment go on to experience partial relapse of symptoms (Eisen et al., 2013). Unfavorable exposure treatment outcomes are attributed to multiple factors. This includes factors common to many treatments, such as poor adherence to between-session work (Ojalehto et al., 2020), and those more specific to EX/RP’s underlying classical conditioning framework, such as failure of within-session learning generalizing outside of the therapy context and the insufficiency of habituation to a feared stimulus (Jacoby & Abramowitz, 2016). Due to these known difficulties related to EX/RP, as well as the associative learning underpinnings of exposure interventions in general, both basic and clinical scientists have proposed increased investigation into perturbed classical conditioning and extinction processes in OCD as a potential route forward for further innovation and improvement in EX/RP and related therapies (e.g., Foa & McLean, 2016; Jacoby & Abramowitz, 2016).
1.1. OCD and Classical Conditioning and Extinction Principles
Classical fear conditioning (Pavlov, 1927) is the process through which an inherently neutral unconditioned stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus (CS) through repeated pairings with an inherently aversive unconditioned stimulus (US). Through acquisition of the aversive CS-US association, the CS is capable of eliciting a conditioned response (CR), such as defensive reactions, increased physiological responses, and subjective reports of fear in humans. Following acquisition of the CR, the CR can be diminished through repeated presentations of the CS in the absence of the US (i.e., extinction). Both conditioning and extinction learning are increasingly well-understood across animal species at both the behavioral and neural levels (Fullana et al., 2016, 2018; Maren, 2001; Vervliet & Boddez, 2020). Conditioning and extinction offer a compelling and testable framework for understanding anxiety-related disorders, as they describe fundamental mechanisms through which people acquire severe anxiety that does not abate over time or through new experiences (Mineka & Zinbarg, 2006; Vervliet, Craske, et al., 2013).
The components of classical conditioning are evident in established behavioral models of OCD (Rachman & Hodgson, 1980; Teasdale, 1974) (see Figure 1). For example, the CR can take the form of distressing obsessions that arise from observing, experiencing, or imagining a negative consequence (US) related to an object or situation (CS). Examples include a patient seeing a family member cut themselves on a knife the patient owns or imagining stabbing the family member, or the patient becoming sick after touching an unclean surface. Further, the obsessions are not limited to the period around the initial experience of the CS and US together and often continue unabated even when the US is not present, such as when the patient’s family member uses a knife but does not sustain an injury. This is analogous to impaired extinction learning, such that the CR does not diminish in the absence of the US. In some circumstances, the obsessions can serve as the CS itself, as the obsessional content leads to subsequent thoughts that are also distressing. For example, some patients think about a family member being hurt and then think, “I am evil for wanting that to happen.” Obsessions might inevitably gain distressing properties through this type of circuitous process (Salkovskis, 1985). Obsessional content and distress can also generalize to neutral stimuli that incidentally resemble the initial CS. In the case of a knife that caused a family member harm, generalization might lead to other bladed objects or household tools becoming CSs. Compulsions, such as constantly checking if dangerous objects are locked away, can temporarily reduce obsessions but circumvent the opportunity for exposure and lead to the return of maladaptive and unwanted thoughts and behavior (Meyer, 1966). In other words, extinction of the CR does not occur and there is only a temporary reduction. Further, compulsions are more likely to be enacted in the future due to strengthened associations with temporary relief from distress (i.e., negative reinforcement; Mowrer, 1947). Although the described conditioning model does not fully explain the phenomenology of OCD (see work on cognitive models, e.g., Abramowitz et al., 2007; Salkovskis, 1999; Taylor et al., 2007), it provides a key foundation for systematic empirical investigation into the pathological substrates of the disorder and offers considerable promise for productive clinical translational efforts.
Figure 1.
Schematic of classical conditioning processes within the context of OCD and in the laboratory setting. Top half of figure models the development of obsessions and defensive responding. Note that in this example, the initial distressing experience could be observing actual harm or imagining harming others. Bottom half of figure shows the corresponding components of a classical conditioning protocol that uses electric shock as the US. CS = conditioned stimulus; CS+ = conditioned threat cue; SCR = skin conductance response; US = unconditioned stimulus.
1.2. The Current Review
To date, there has not been a systematic review of conditioning work in OCD, and thus there is not currently a synthesis of the literature to serve as a foundation for future research. In particular, it is not clear if impaired acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear are characteristic of OCD and, if so, what form or pattern these deficits take. Synthesizing this literature and drawing cross-study conclusions is particularly needed before the important next step of leveraging experimental conditioning work to improve treatment for OCD, especially as exciting new avenues for extinction-based treatments, such as behavioral and pharmaceutical extinction enhancers, emerge from pre-clinical research (for reviews, see Craske et al., 2014; Dunsmoor et al., 2015). In the current review, we address this need by systematically reviewing the OCD and conditioning literature to determine if aberrant acquisition and extinction processes are consistently related to OCD across studies. We also evaluate design differences and experimental and assessment limitations that potentially hinder strong conclusions, with a goal of providing recommendations for future work in this area. We limit the scope of our review to studies using associative conditioning paradigms employing aversive USs. Empirical interest in conditioning and anxiety-related disorders primarily focuses on aversive conditioning, and current OCD treatments place a large emphasis on the reduction of unpleasant emotional experiences and the behaviors they motivate. We also limit reviewed articles to studies of classical (i.e., Pavlovian) aversive conditioning and extinction in OCD. Currently, there are not a sufficient number of published studies to allow for a comprehensive review of other forms of conditioning (e.g., operant conditioning) as they relate to OCD.
We first provide an overview of the methodological characteristics of identified studies and then provide a detailed narrative review of key findings from each study. We next briefly summarize acquisition and extinction results across studies, discuss the findings from our review in the context of the larger conditioning and OCD literature, and conclude with a discussion of broad next steps and specific future recommendations in this area.
2. Method
Our systematic review was conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA; Moher et al., 2009) statement. The documents used during literature search and screening for this review are available via this project’s repository on the Open Source Framework (https://osf.io/a6wm4/).
2.1. Literature Search Strategy
Our strategy was designed to find published empirical work that 1) applied a classical conditioning protocol 2) using an aversive US 3) to test people with and without OCD or those with differing levels of OCD-related traits (e.g., personality traits that are empirically demonstrated to relate to OCD, but also are present to some degree in most people), including some with elevated levels that are consistent with OCD pathology, and 4) reported between-group differences or correlational analyses with an OCD scale on one or more conditioning index. We did not exclude eyeblink conditioning, which resembles basic classical conditioning except that the US is an air-puff to the eye and the CR is the eyeblink that is elicited by the anticipation of the air-puff. We included these studies because of the anticipated paucity of relevant OCD studies and because eyeblink conditioning is somewhat aversive (e.g., fear-relevant circuitry is involved in the acquisition of the eyeblink CR; Ng & Freeman, 2014). We limited the scope of this review to human research (for reviews of OCD-related research in non-human animals, see Abramowitz et al., 2011; Ahmari & Dougherty, 2015; Boulougouris et al., 2009; Szechtman et al., 2017).
All searches were performed using Google Scholar and Pubmed. We searched article titles, abstracts, and full text for the following keywords: (OCD OR obsessive-compulsive OR obsessive OR compulsive) AND (conditioning OR conditioned AND fear OR disgust OR aversive OR classical OR Pavlovian OR extinction OR acquisition OR differential OR evaluative) OR (associative learning) AND human AND NOT review. We also screened the references of articles from these searches, from recent reviews of exposure therapy in OCD that consider associative learning principles (Arch & Abramowitz, 2015; Jacoby & Abramowitz, 2016), and from two meta-analyses of classical conditioning in anxiety-related disorders (Duits et al., 2015; Lissek et al., 2005) for additional articles that met criteria for our review. Identified domain experts were also queried for possible eligible articles. The final search for this review was conducted on December 02, 2020.
2.2. Screening
We screened identified articles using the following a priori inclusion criteria: 1) original data from an aversive conditioning paradigm was reported for a group with OCD or elevated traits closely associated with OCD; 2) a validated diagnostic measure of OCD,psychometric measure of OCD symptoms, or a measure of OCD-related traits was administered; 3) English language. After initial searches, tightened search criteria were re-applied to titles and abstracts only to ensure non-relevant incidental matches (e.g., match only found in reference section) were excluded. Screening was done by the first author (SC) and confirmed with the senior author (JD).
3. Results
3.1. Characteristics of Identified Studies
After full-text review, screening resulted in a total of twelve studies that met eligibility out of 6,190 from the initial search (see Figure 2 for PRSIMA flow diagram). Of these studies, two used a single-cue design (Table 1; No. 11, 12; Nanbu et al., 2010; Tracy et al., 1999), which only involve one CS (termed a CS+) that is paired with the US during the acquisition phase. The remaining ten studies used a differential conditioning design, which includes CS+ trials as well as a CS that is never paired with the US (CS−). Six of these studies used a differential cue design (Table 1; No. 1, 2, 4, 5, 7, 8, ; Apergis-Schoute et al., 2017; Armstrong & Olatunji, 2017; Geller et al., 2017, 2019; Kaczkurkin & Lissek, 2013; McGuire et al., 2016). The remaining four studies used a differential cue-in-context design, in which the CSs were superimposed over a background context picture that varied between different phases of the experiment (Table 1; No. 3, 6, 9, 10; Fyer et al., 2020; Giménez et al., 2020; McLaughlin et al., 2015; Milad et al., 2013). Nine studies included at least one phase in which the CS+ was no longer reinforced, as most commonly done in extinction learning phases (Table 1; No. 1–6, 8–12; Apergis-Schoute et al., 2017; Armstrong & Olatunji, 2017; Fyer et al., 2020; Geller et al., 2017, 2019; Giménez et al., 2020; McGuire et al., 2016; McLaughlin et al., 2015; Milad et al., 2013; Nanbu et al., 2010; Tracy et al., 1999). Three studies tested children (as opposed to adults) (Table 1; No. 4, 5, 8; Geller et al., 2017, 2019; McGuire et al., 2016). Eleven studies used a tactile or auditory aversive US, and one study used disgust-inducing images as the US (Table 1; 2; Armstrong & Olatunji, 2017). Seven studies tested a group of participants with a current diagnosis of OCD (Table 1; No. 1, 3–6, 8, 10, 11; Apergis-Schoute et al., 2017; Fyer et al., 2020; Geller et al., 2017, 2019; Giménez et al., 2020; McGuire et al., 2016; Milad et al., 2013; Nanbu et al., 2010), one tested those with a lifetime, but not necessarily current, diagnosis of OCD (Table 1; No. 9; McLaughlin et al., 2015)1, and the remaining three created OCD analogue groups (for review on this practice, see Abramowitz et al., 2014) for between-group comparisons (Table 1; 2, 7, 12; Armstrong & Olatunji, 2017; Kaczkurkin & Lissek, 2013; Tracey et al., 1999). Sample sizes ranged from N=30 to N=105. Most studies reported an N=64 or less; only three studies analyzed larger samples (Table 1; No. 1, 3, 4; Apergis-Schoute et al., 2017; Fyer et al., 2020; Geller et al., 2017). In studies comparing OCD groups to control groups, the ratio of OCD to control participants ranged from 0.64 to 1.87.
Fig 2.
PRISMA flow diagram
Table 1.
Characteristics and primary results of N = 12 studies included in systematic review.
No. | Study Name | Participants and Clinical Characteristics | Paradigm | CR Measure | Primary Significant Results (by Phase) | ||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N (OCD/Control)1 | Age Range | Clinical Status | Primary Diagnostic, Symptom, and Trait Assessments | Diagnostic Comorbidities Allowed? | Medications | Design | ACQ (Day 1) | EXT (Day 1) | REC (Day 2) | REN (Day 2) | CSs | CTXs | US | CS - US contingency | Behavioral | Functional Neuroimaging | |||
1 | Apergis-Schoute et al., 2017 | 43 / 35 | Adult | Clinical |
Diagnostic:
MINI Symptom: YBOCS |
No | Majority SSRI, also benzodiazepines, atypical antidepressants, antipsychotics | Differential cue | ✓ | ✓ 2 | 2 mildly angry male faces | shock | 33% | SCR, fMRI |
ACQ: elevated SCR to CS+ in OCD
during later trials. Reversal: elevated SCR to CS+ in OCD continues after reversal (when former CS+ is extinguished). |
CS+>CS−
contrasts ACQ: OCD > Control in vmPFC during first half. Increased functional connectivity between vmPFC and salience network in OCD. Reversal: OCD > Control in left caudate and left insula during second half. |
|||
2 | Armstrong & Olatunji, 2017 | 32 / 30 | Adult | Non-clinical | Trait: PI (contamination subscale) | Not assessed | Not assessed | Differential cue | ✓ | ✓ | 2 neutral male faces | disgust images | 100% | Retrospective disgust and US expectancy ratings |
ACQ: elevated disgust ratings to
CS+ in higher OCD trait group. EXT: elevated disgust ratings to CS+ in higher OCD trait group continues after extinction training. US expectancy ratings do not change from acquisition ratings in high OCD trait group. |
||||
3 | Fyer et al., 2020 | 41/64 | Adult | Clinical |
Diagnostic: DSM3 Symptom: YBOCS |
Yes | Unmedicated | Differential cue-in-context | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | 2 lamp color pictures | 2 rooms | shock | 67% | SCR |
ACQ: elevated SCR to CS−
in OCD across the entire phase. Elevated SCR to CS+ in OCD for specific
trials. EXT: none REC: none. REN: poorer (larger) SCR discrimination in the OCD group. |
|
4 | Geller et al., 2017 | 39 / 41 | Youth (8–18) | Clinical |
Diagnostic:
KSADS-PL Symptoms: CY-BOCS |
Yes | SSRIs only | Differential cue | ✓ | ✓ | 2 neutral female faces | scream + fearful face | 80% | SCR |
ACQ: elevated SCR to CS+ in
OCD. EXT: continued elevated SCR to CS+ in OCD. |
||||
5 | Geller et al., 2019 | 64 / 0 | Youth (7–17) | Clinical |
Diagnostic:
KSADS-PL Symptoms: CY-BOCS |
Yes | Majority SSRI, also stimulants and antipsychotics | Differential cue | ✓ | ✓ | 2 neutral female faces | scream + fearful face | 80% | SCR |
ACQ:
none. EXT: none. |
||||
6 | Giménez et al., 2020 | 17/13 | Adult | Clinical |
Diagnostic:
SCID Symptom: YBOCS |
Yes | SSRIs only | Differential cue-in-context | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | 3 lamp color pictures | 2 rooms | shock | 60% | SCR |
ACQ:
none. EXT: none. |
||
7 | Kaczkurkin & Lissek, 2013 | 28 / 31 | Adult | Non-clinical |
Symptom:
OCI-R Trait: OBQ |
Not assessed | Not assessed | Differential cue | ✓ 4 | large/small rings + intermediary rings | shock | 75% | FPS, online risk ratings |
ACQ:
none. Generalization: elevated generalization of FPS in higher OCD trait group. |
|||||
8 | McGuire et al., 2016 | 19 / 22 | Youth (7–17) | Clinical |
Diagnostic:
KSADS-PL Symptoms: CY-BOCS |
Yes | Majority SSRI, also benzodiazepines, stimulants, antipsychotics | Differential cue | ✓ | ✓ | 2 neutral female faces | scream + fear face | 80% | SCR |
ACQ: OCD symptoms positively
correlated with SCR to CS+ EXT: elevated SCR to CS+ in OCD group. |
||||
9 | McLaughlin et al., 2015 | 31 / 18 | Adult | Clinical5 |
Diagnostic:
SCID Symptom: YBOCS |
Yes | SSRIs only | Differential cue-in-context | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | 2 lamp color pictures | 2 rooms | shock | 100% | SCR |
ACQ:
none. EXT: none. REC: poorer (larger) SCR discrimination in the OCD group. Poorer (smaller) % of extinction training retained in OCD group. REN: none. |
|
10 | Milad et al., 2013 | 21 / 21 | Adult | Clinical |
Diagnostic:
SCID Symptom: YBOCS |
Yes | Majority SSRI, also benzodiazepines, atypical antidepressants, antipsychotics | Differential cue-in-context | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | 3 lamp color pictures | 2 rooms | shock | 60% | SCR, fMRI |
ACQ:
none. EXT: none. REC: poorer (larger) SCR discrimination in the OCD group. Poorer (smaller) % of extinction training retained in OCD group. |
CS+ > CS−
contrasts ACQ: OCD < Control in caudate, hippocampus, vmPFC. EXT: OCD < Control in vmPFC. REC: OCD < Control in cerebellum, PCC, putamen, vmPFC. |
|
11 | Nanbu et al., 2010 | 39 / 21 | Adult | Clinical |
Diagnostic:
SCID Symptom: YBOCS |
No | SSRIs only | Single-cue cue | ✓ | ✓ | lightbulb on/off | shock | 100% | SCR, EEG (P50 ERP) |
ACQ:
none. EXT: none. |
ACQ:
none. EXT: elevated P50 ratio in OCD group. |
|||
12 | Tracy et al., 1999 | 20 / 22 | Adult | Non-clinical | Symptom: MOCI | Not assessed | Not assessed | Single-cue cue | ✓ | ✓ | lightbulb on/off | air-puff | 90% | eyeblink |
ACQ: quicker acquisition of CR to
CS+ in higher OCD trait group. EXT: none. |
Abbreviations: ACQ = acquisition; CS = conditioned stimulus; CS+ = conditioned threat cue; CS− = conditioned safety cue; CR = conditioned response; CTX = context; CY-BOCS = Children’s Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale; DSM = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual; EEG = electroencephalography; ERP = event related potential; EXT = extinction training; fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging; FPS = fear potentiated startle; KSADS-PL = Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia - Present and Lifetime; MINI = Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inventory; MOCI = Maudsley Obsessional Compulsive Inventory; OBQ = Obsessive Beliefs Questionnaire; OC = obsessive-compulsive; OCD = obsessive-compulsive disorder; OCI-R = Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder - Revised; PCC = posterior cingulate cortex; PI = Padua Inventory; REC = extinction recall; REN = extinction renewal; SCR = skin conductance response; SSRI = selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor; US = unconditioned stimulus; vmPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
Here, OCD refers to both diagnosed OCD and groups designated as OCD-analogues (e.g., high OCD traits or symptoms).
Extinction training in the context of a Reversal phase (i.e., in addition to omission of US with CS+ presentations, previous CS− is now paired with the US).
OCD diagnosis established using DSM criteria by a clinician as part of the study assessment or by medical record.
A generalization phase followed the acquisition phase for this study.
Includes participants with either lifetime or current OCD diagnoses.
3.2. Detailed Narrative Review of Identified Studies
Given a limited number of studies that met criteria for inclusion in this review, we can provide a detailed narrative review of each study. Our detailed review is comprised of two subsections: acquisition of behavioral CRs (e.g., physiological arousal, self-report ratings) and extinction of behavioral CRs. We distinguish between extinction learning (i.e., extinction training or within-session extinction) and tests of extinction retention, such as extinction recall or contextual renewal. Common methodological variations or extensions of acquisition (e.g., generalization) or extinction processes (e.g., reversal) are included in the relevant subsection. Note that most studies are reviewed in both the acquisition and extinction sections, as all studies with an extinction component contained a preceding acquisition procedure. Additionally, three studies included in this review (Apergis-Schoute et al., 2017; Milad et al., 2013; Nanbu et al., 2010) reported both behavioral and functional neuroimaging (electroencephalography [EEG] or functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI]) results. These studies’ neuroimaging results are separately discussed in a subsequent section (see 3.2.3 Functional Neuroimaging Studies).
3.2.1. Acquisition of Behavioral CRs
Of the twelve identified studies, seven report at least one significant acquisition deficit or impairment related to OCD (see Table 1). One of these studies, conducted by Nanbu et al. (2010), was a single-cue study that used a shock US and measured SCR as one conditioning index. There were no between-group differences in SCR indices for the CS+ during the acquisition phase. The other single-cue study (Tracy et al., 1999) used eyeblink conditioning (air-puff US and eyeblink CR). There was a significant between-group difference at acquisition, with those higher on a composite of OCD-related traits more rapidly acquiring CRs to the CS+ (i.e., response potentiates within first few trials) compared with participants lower on these traits. However, more rapid acquisition might not correspond to a maladaptive or abnormal response (e.g., Cook et al., 1985; Rescorla, 1988). Thus, it is not clear if this finding can be considered an OCD-associated acquisition deficit. Tracy et al. (1999) also did not find between-group differences in the strength of the CRs. Considering both single-cue studies together, they do not appear to provide strong evidence for enhanced acquisition related to OCD. For a detailed description of the drawbacks of single cue designs, see Lissek et al. 2005.
Three studies (Geller et al., 2017, 2019; McGuire et al., 2016) tested children with OCD (ages 7–18). These studies used a differential (CS+ and CS−) conditioning design with neutrally expressive female faces as CSs and an auditory scream as the US. Geller et al. (2017) reported significantly greater SCRs to the CS+ in those with OCD compared with control participants. Using the same design, McGuire et al. (2016) reported a positive correlation between a dimensional scale of OCD symptoms and SCR to the CS+. However, Geller et al. (2019) did not find a significant correlation between a dimensional OCD symptom measure and acquisition indices, which is inconsistent with McGuire and colleagues’ (2016) correlational findings. A possible explanation is related to McGuire et al. (2016) including both those with and without an OCD diagnosis in their correlational analyses. In contrast, the sample in Geller et al. (2019) was only comprised of children who met criteria for OCD during testing. Reported OCD symptom scores had relatively moderate variance and, therefore, possibly limited range. Thus, attenuated range of and decreased variance in OCD symptom scores in Geller et al. (2019) might have impacted the strength of the association.
In a study of adults with OCD, Aspergis-Schoute et al. (2017) used mildly angry male faces as CSs, a shock as the US, and SCR as one index of differential conditioning. In this study, early trials and late trials of the acquisition phase were analyzed separately. Those in the OCD group showed poorer discrimination (i.e., smaller differential SCRs between CS+ and CS−) compared with the control group during later, but not earlier, acquisition trials. Poor discrimination is sometimes taken to reflect a failure to learn safety signals relative to threat and is more characteristic of anxiety-related disorder pathology than single-cue reactivity (Duits et al., 2015; Lissek et al., 2005). Their follow-up analyses showed that this effect was driven by greater responding to the CS+ in the OCD group relative to controls, indicating that safety learning was not necessarily aberrant in the OCD group.
Two studies conducted by Milad and colleagues (McLaughlin et al., 2015; Milad et al., 2013), as well as studies by Fyer et al. (2020) and Giménez et al. (2020), also tested adults with OCD using a shock US. These four studies employed cue-in-context designs, with lamp colors as the cue CSs and pictures of different rooms used as contexts. Three of these did not find differences in conditioned SCRs between OCD and control groups during the acquisition phase. The exception was Fyer et al. (2020), which found significantly greater SCR to the CS− in the OCD group compared with control participants across the whole acquisition phase. When breaking this down at the trial level, however, CS+ responses to all but the initial trial are greater in the OCD group compared with the control group. The mixed nature of the findings from this set of studies, with the majority failing to find significant between-group acquisition differences, accords with earlier studies using this cue-in-context task in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or other anxiety-related disorders that did not find any differences in acquisition-related SCRs between disorder and comparison groups (e.g., Milad et al., 2009; Shvil et al., 2014).
No differences during acquisition were observed in fear-potentiated startle (FPS; a measure of central nervous system-mediated defensive responding; Davis, 1992) or subjective ratings of risk of shock between individuals characterized as high or low on OCD symptoms and related traits (Kaczkurkin & Lissek, 2013). This study followed the acquisition phase with a second phase that introduced generalization stimuli (GSs; in this study represented by rings of intermediary sizes between the CS+ and CS−), along with continued presentation of the CS+ (intermittingly reinforced to prevent extinction) and CS−. Those with high levels of OCD-related threat overestimation demonstrated higher FPS, but not risk ratings, to the GSs most resembling the CS+ (i.e., fear generalized from the CS+ to the most visually similar stimuli) compared with those low on this OCD-related trait. The process of acquiring fear to GSs overlaps with the process of CS+/CS− discrimination and is mediated by shared neurocircuitry (Hermans et al., 2013; Lissek, 2012). Accordingly, these generalization results can be interpreted as poorer discrimination learning in those with elevated levels of an OCD-related threat overestimation. They are also roughly comparable to other results in the current review that found poorer discrimination learning associated with OCD.
Armstrong and Olatunji (2017) employed a differential cue design using neutral male faces as CSs and disgust-related images as US2. Although disgust is a core emotion involved in OCD pathology (e.g., Cisler et al., 2009; Olatunji, Lohr, et al., 2007), there are only two studies examining disgust conditioning within the broad context of OCD-related constructs (Armstrong & Olatunji, 2017; Olatunji et al., 2017), with only one meeting our inclusion criteria. In Armstrong and Olatunji (2017), groups were constructed based on a dimensional measure of contamination concerns (a preoccupation with becoming infected or dirtied by outside contaminants that has been strongly linked to OCD pathology; Brady et al., 2010), with a higher scoring group framed as an analogue-OCD group. The CRs were ratings of subjective level of disgust for the CS as well as expectancy for receiving the US. Ratings were collected after each phase rather than simultaneously with conditioning. Participants with higher contamination concern rated the CS+ as significantly more disgust-provoking than those in the low group. Although this result superficially aligns with other findings of enhanced CRs to the CS+ in OCD, caution should be taken in considering this an equivalent finding due to the nature of the CR measurement in this study (a retrospective, single-trial report of perceived disgust). There were no group differences in US expectancy, which was interpreted as indicating that cognitively-mediated processes (compared with the purportedly affective process that the disgust rating represents) are not aberrant in those with higher contamination concerns. As only one of the other reviewed studies included a comparable dependent variable (online risk ratings in Kaczkurkin & Lissek, 2013), it is difficult to ascertain if this reflects a difference between disgust and fear conditioning.
Altogether, there is mixed evidence in the literature that OCD is associated with abnormal acquisition of conditioned fear. In the acquisition phase in single-cue and differential conditioning designs, the relationship between the CS+ and US is mostly unambiguous and a CR represents a normative response to a threat cue. Therefore, it is possible that mixed findings emerge due to a greater chance of uniform responding across all participants irrespective of diagnostic status or symptoms (i.e., a strong situation, Lissek et al., 2006). In the extinction procedure, the contingency becomes more ambiguous due to the omission of the US, and therefore extinction performance is perhaps better suited to detect meaningful differences between OCD and control groups.
3.2.2. Extinction of Behavioral CRs
Eleven of twelve studies included an extinction learning phase immediately after acquisition on the same day (see Table 1). On the day after initial testing, four studies included an extinction recall test, and two of these also included an extinction renewal test. We first review single-day studies that only tested extinction learning and then review the subset of two-day studies that tested extinction learning and then extinction recall (and renewal, for some) the next day.
3.2.2.1. Single-Day Studies of Extinction Learning
Neither single-cue conditioning study found behavioral differences during extinction learning between OCD and control groups (Nanbu et al., 2010; Tracy et al., 1999). Prior research that directly compared single-cue to differential extinction learning observed relatively quick and robust behavioral CR decreases in the single-cue condition (Norrholm et al., 2008) compared with the differential condition. The single-cue design might therefore not provide enough variance in extinction learning CRs to detect OCD-control group effects.
In studies of children with OCD, two studies identified extinction learning abnormalities related to OCD pathology. In McGuire et al. (2016), the OCD group maintained higher SCRs to the CS+ (similar in magnitude to the level of SCRs to the CS+ during the acquisition phase) throughout the extinction learning phase compared with control participants, which suggested a failure to extinguish. Additionally, both groups showed relatively poor discrimination between CS+ and CS−, but control participants showed slightly less discrimination than those with OCD and also demonstrated lower CRs to both stimuli. Unlike acquisition, stronger discrimination is indicative of impaired learning during extinction learning, as the CR is expected to diminish over the course of the extinction learning phase. The authors suggested that maintaining a CR to both the CS+ and CS− is related to participants’ belief that the US contingency will switch from one CS to the other, which has been observed in past conditioning studies in children (e.g., Jovanovic et al., 2014). Geller et al. (2017), which used the same task, also concluded that extinction learning is impaired in OCD. One subtle distinction between Geller et al. (2017) and McGuire (2016) is that the former found a Group × Stimulus effect for the entire extinction phase, whereas the latter only found a group difference in later trials. Finally, Geller et al. (2019) did not find any evidence of impaired extinction learning in children with OCD.
Apergis-Schoute et al. (2017) used a reversal learning design following the acquisition phase, and therefore this study is comparable to other studies using standard extinction learning designs. In reversal learning, the acquisition CS+ is no longer paired with the US (i.e., it is extinguished), while at the same time, the previously unpaired CS− is now paired with the US. As measured by SCR, participants with OCD were impaired at learning the new associations for the CS+ and CS− relative to control participants, with the OCD group showing decreased discrimination between the CS+ and CS− across the entire reversal phase. These reversal learning results align with the other reviewed results that implicate impaired extinction learning is associated with OCD. Additionally, reversal learning involves both extinction learning and acquiring fear to a previously safe stimulus and is conceptualized as a probe of flexible learning (Schiller et al., 2008). Accordingly, a deficit in reversal learning in OCD potentially indicates that the ability to adjust responding to new fear and safety information in a flexible manner is impaired in the disorder, as shown in PTSD (Homan et al., 2019).
The single available study of disgust conditioning (Armstrong & Olatunji, 2017) reported impaired extinction learning in a sample with elevated OCD-related traits. Specifically, a group with elevated contamination concerns demonstrated impaired extinction relative to a group with lower contamination concerns on measures of disgust and US expectancy. US-expectancy ratings were overall elevated for the higher group, whereas disgust ratings were specifically elevated for the CS+ relative to the CS−. Further, the change in US-expectancy for the CS+ from acquisition to extinction was significantly smaller for the higher group; this effect was not observed for disgust ratings. Notably, these ratings were collected retroactively after the extinction phase. These results align with extinction results from the reviewed studies using non-disgust USs, albeit with dependent measures collected retrospectively following extinction. An additional consideration for interpretation of disgust learning data is the possibility that non-classical conditioning processes contributed to extinction results. Evaluative conditioning (learning to like or dislike a stimulus; De Houwer et al., 2001) is implicated in disgust learning (e.g., Olatunji et al., 2009) and is more resistant to extinction procedures than classically conditioned fear (e.g., Blechert et al., 2008; Olatunji et al., 2007; Vansteenwegen et al., 2006). If interpreting through the lens of evaluative conditioning, difficulty extinguishing in the higher contamination group might reflect an initially acquired dislike of the CS+, as opposed to any specific difficulty extinguishing that is explicitly linked with individual differences in contamination concerns. Although we cannot confirm this explanation or not, given that Armstrong and Olatunji (2017) did not collect evaluative ratings, it remains a viable alternative explanation and caution is recommended when synthesizing these results with the fear conditioning studies of OCD.
3.2.2.2. Two-Day Studies of Extinction Learning and Recall
All the reviewed two-day studies (Fyer et al., 2020; Giménez et al., 2020; McLaughlin et al., 2015; Milad et al., 2013) applied a cue-in-context paradigm to adults with OCD. These studies included a second day of testing to test for recall of the extinction memory. Compared with immediate testing of extinction learning, extinction recall tests provide insight into the retention of extinction learning, which is often impaired in anxiety-related disorders (Milad & Quirk, 2012). In these cue-in-context designs, the context for both extinction learning and extinction recall incorporated the same background picture (referred to as an ABB design).
None of these four studies found evidence for extinction learning deficits in OCD, and all reported significant extinction results were found during day-two testing. In Milad et al. (2013), impaired extinction recall was operationalized with two metrics: differential SCRs between the CS+ and CS− across the first two recall trials, and an “extinction retention index” that quantified extinction recall as the magnitude of SCR during recall relative to initial extinction. Higher values on this index indicated increased maintenance of extinction learning. In this study, differential SCR analyses yielded limited evidence for impaired extinction recall in the OCD group, which showed overall greater SCR during this phase, but without a significant difference in CS+ vs. CS− magnitudes. In a group comparison, the extinction retention index showed that the OCD group failed to maintain initial extinction levels relative to the control group. Conversely and surprisingly, improved extinction retention was positively correlated with the degree of OCD symptom severity, which was at odds with the hypothesis that extinction learning would not persist in those with OCD. The study by McLaughlin et al. (2015), which also assessed extinction recall, replicated the primary group difference findings from Milad et al. (2013). However, in this study, extinction retention index scores were not significantly correlated with OCD symptom scores, which is different from Milad and colleagues’ (2013) findings. Extinction recall differences in Fyer et al. (2020) were limited to significant differential SCR responding, with elevated CS+ responses, for a subset of trials within the OCD group. This pattern was not seen in the control group, but the direct statistical comparison between the two groups was not significant. Similarly, the extinction retention index was not significantly different between groups. The remaining study, Giménez et al. (2020), did not find any significant between-group differences during the extinction recall phase. The mixed nature of extinction recall and retention results here is reminiscent of a broader pattern in the literature, particularly as these processes relate to individual differences (Lonsdorf et al., 2017; Lonsdorf & Merz, 2017). Also notable is that operationalization of recall and retention varies across studies, most notably in regards to the number of trials that are analyzed. Inconsistent operationalization of recall and retention are identified as contributing to ambiguity and failure to replicate across studies (Lonsdorf et al., 2019).
McLaughlin et al. (2015) and Fyer et al. (2020) included an additional phase after extinction recall that tested contextual fear renewal (i.e., return of CR related to an incidental change in context without reintroducing the US; Bouton, 2004). During renewal, the background context was the same as the one used during the acquisition phase (referred to as an ABA renewal design). Fyer et al. (2020) found the OCD group demonstrated significantly greater CS+/CS− differential SCRs compared with the control group, with CS+ SCRs in the OCD group consistently elevated across the entire renewal phase. Conversely, there were no group differences in renewal in McLaughlin et al. (2015), as both groups demonstrated similarly elevated SCRs to both CS+ and CS− during early trials that gradually decreased. A possible explanation for the inconsistent group differences in renewal is that context at test was the same background used when the CS was paired with shock, which might generate a heightened expectation of threat in both groups that weakens the potential between-group difference. For this reason, additional research on fear renewal in OCD is warranted that perhaps compares contextual renewal in a novel environment (referred to as an ABC renewal design) rather than in the acquisition or extinction context (for reviews, see Bouton, 2002; Vervliet et al., 2013). It is also important to note that the OCD group in McLaughlin et al. (2015) was less symptomatic than in Fyer et al. (2020) and that participants all had a lifetime, but not necessarily current, OCD diagnosis. Accordingly, it is possible that significant renewal differences are contingent on higher severity of current OCD symptoms.
3.2.3. Functional Neuroimaging Studies
To date, only three studies have used functional neuroimaging to examine associative fear learning processes in OCD. Neuroimaging of OCD, at a broader level, has tended to focus on a set of orbitofrontal-striatal interactions (or “fronto-striatal loops”) that operate in parallel function and appear dysregulated in OCD (Breiter & Rauch, 1996; Graybiel & Rauch, 2000; Menzies et al., 2008; Saxena & Rauch, 2000). These circuits contribute to a broad suite of behavioral control functions and consist of projections from orbitofrontal regions associated with a range of higher-order operations, such as affect regulation (e.g., ventromedial prefrontal cortex [vmPFC]) and executive functioning (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [dlPFC]), to striatal reward and action regions (e.g., nucleus accumbens, caudate, putamen), which then project back to the frontal regions via the thalamus (Alexander et al., 1986). These regions have also been meta-analytically implicated as underlying emotional processing aberrations in other anxiety-related disorders (Etkin & Wager, 2007). Milad and Rauch (2012) linked the fronto-striatal model of OCD to a fear-conditioning neural model that centers on the amygdala, hippocampus, and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) (Fullana et al., 2018; Milad et al., 2007). This aligns with meta-analytic findings that regions implicated in fear extinction, namely the amygdala and ACC (data was insufficient to implicate the dACC subdivision), are impaired in OCD (Thorsen et al., 2018).
Milad et al. (2013) found that during acquisition, participants with OCD demonstrated decreased fMRI activity (relative to control participants) to the CS+ (relative to the CS−) in the hippocampus, caudate, and vmPFC. All these regions are involved in fear conditioning, with the vmPFC receiving particular attention due to its relation to safety learning (Milad & Quirk, 2012). During extinction, vmPFC activity to the CS+ (relative to CS) remained lower for the OCD group relative to control participants. The extinction recall stage yielded the strongest collection of findings in favor of deficits related to OCD: the vmPFC was hypoactive in OCD relative to control participants, as were the cerebellum (implicated in behavioral response related to conditioning processes, e.g., Thompson & Steinmetz, 2009), the putamen region within the striatum, and the posterior cingulate cortex (a region implicated in mediating internally-focused cognition and found to be related to impaired inhibition in OCD, e.g., Del Casale et al., 2011; Leech & Sharp, 2014).
Apergis-Schoute et al. (2017) also employed fMRI and found vmPFC hyperactivity in the OCD group in response to the CS+ (relative to CS−) in the first, but not the second, half of the acquisition phase. This finding is striking because one of the most consistent findings from univariate contrasts of differential fear conditioning in fMRI is greater activity to the unpaired (i.e., safe) CS− versus the CS+ (i.e., CS− > CS+) in healthy adults (Fullana et al., 2016). That the OCD group exhibited the opposite result (i.e., CS+ > CS−) indicates an absence of safety signaling in the vmPFC, at least during early learning as participants attempted to learn which CS predicted danger and which CS predicted the absence of danger. Task-related functional connectivity analysis during early acquisition, using the vmPFC as the seed region, revealed multiple areas within the functionally-defined salience network (Uddin, 2014) were more strongly co-activated in the OCD group on CS+ trials. These areas included dACC, bilateral insula, and right thalamus. Stronger task-related connectivity between vmPFC and these salience network nodes in participants with OCD was interpreted as biased processing towards threat at the expense of safety processing. During late reversal, the left caudate and left insula tracked the new CS+ (differential CS+ > CS−) better in the control group than in the OCD group. These results highlight that OCD is characterized by impaired fear and safety updating in brain regions putatively involved in tracking the threat and safety value of conditioned stimuli.
Nanbu et al. (2010) used EEG in conjunction with auditory probes to measure P50 event-related potential as a CR. The P50 response is indexed during CS+ presentations by calculating the response ratio between an initial and subsequent auditory probe. The P50 is interpreted as a deficit in sensory-gating processes and proposed to index enhanced cue conditioning. There were no between-group differences in the P50 ratio during acquisition. However, during extinction, participants with OCD had significantly higher P50 ratios on CS+ trials compared with control participants. This difference was interpreted as increased difficulty inhibiting sensory processing during extinction for those with OCD, which the authors link to poor inhibition of intrusive thoughts and obsessions.
4. Discussion
Based on this review of the literature of fear conditioning and extinction in OCD, results implicate mixed evidence for impaired fear acquisition but compelling evidence for impaired extinction in OCD. Acquisition and extinction deficits were apparent across different types of self-report, behavioral, and neurobiological indicators. Amassed results also suggest that these deficits are detectable in both children and adults with OCD, in sub-clinical OCD and analog groups, and are associated with symptom-level features of the disorder. Limited evidence also supports the assertion that conditioning deficits are not limited to fear-related processing (as a single study tested disgust conditioning). However, this is a tentative conclusion, and more research is warranted, as we discuss below.
4.1. Tentative Evidence for Increased Acquisition of CS+ Response in OCD
Overall, evidence for acquisition deficits in OCD is mixed. Six out of twelve studies reported behavioral evidence and two studies reporting neuroimaging evidence in support of an OCD-associated deficit. Additionally, the specific form of acquisition deficit was not consistent across studies. This heterogeneity of results suggests that impaired acquisition is only weakly associated with OCD, at least in the context of relatively simple conditioning tasks. A defensive CR is also a normative response to impending threat during acquisition, and thus only an abnormally elevated response would likely differentiate an anxiety-related disorder sample from the rest of the population.
The most consistent OCD-associated pattern to emerge from these results was increased CR magnitude to the CS+ without a corresponding increase in CS− response. This specific pattern has been related to the concept of enhanced conditionability (most often discussed in conjunction with PTSD, see Lissek & van Meurs, 2014; Orr et al., 2000). Enhanced conditionability, which broadly refers to a between-subjects difference in speed and magnitude of CS+ acquisition, is not an ideal description for the reviewed results because only one set of results provide evidence of more rapid CS+ acquisition in OCD (Tracy et al., 1999). Speed of acquisition was either not tested in the other studies or did not differ by group. That said, abnormally elevated CS+ responding (irrespective of speed of acquisition) could be consistent with the clinical phenomenology of obsessions in OCD. There is strong evidence that pervasive and repetitive thoughts are common for everyone, not just those who have or go on to develop OCD (García-Soriano et al., 2011; Fullana et al., 2009), which suggests that the mere presence of these types of thoughts are not sufficient for the development of the severe obsessions that are characteristic of OCD. Therefore, an increased tendency to form stronger aversive associations might promote the conversion from normal to maladaptive intrusion/obsession and thus act as both predisposing risk factor and key pathogenic mechanism of OCD. Put another way, two people might have the same intrusive thought over a period of days, but the one who responds with greater distress is more likely to have the intrusion develop into a full obsession and thus develop OCD.
Also consistent with a pattern of increased CS+ responding in OCD are results from a quasi-experimental self-report study that found a general cognitive tendency (indexed by measures of different dysfunctional beliefs) towards overestimating threat in the disorder (for review, see Hezel & McNally, 2016). That said, none of the currently reviewed studies found evidence for increased CS+ responding on more cognitively mediated measures, such as US expectancy or risk ratings, although these measures weren’t assessed in each study. In general, based on the empirical research, it is premature to integrate evidence of broad belief patterns with the physiological and neuroimaging evidence reviewed here. Further, a pattern of increased CS+ responding in differential conditioning paradigms also diverges from some non-conditioning experimental work in OCD. This body of work consistently finds that inhibitory or regulatory, but not excitatory, processes differentiate those with OCD from those without (for reviews, see Cavedini et al., 2006; Milad & Rauch, 2012) and would suggest that CS− responding would also differentiate OCD from control groups. The only reviewed study that aligned with this pattern was Fyer et al. (2020).
In terms of neuroimaging results, the only notable acquisition finding was poorer discrimination between CS+ and CS− in those with OCD within the hippocampus, caudate, and vmPFC in the study by Milad et al. (2013). Although these results do not necessarily contradict the findings of increased CS+ responding as seen in the behavioral data, they also do not explicitly support those findings. That is, poorer discrimination between the CS+ and CS− in OCD does not imply increased activity to the CS+ in these regions relative to control groups. Additionally, the vmPFC is more often associated with safety/inhibitory learning, rather than threat acquisition, in the context of fear conditioning protocols (Fullana et al., 2016).
4.2. Stronger Evidence for Extinction Deficits in OCD
OCD was associated with some form of extinction deficit, either during extinction learning, recall, or contextual renewal, in nine of the eleven studies we reviewed that tested extinction processes. The most prominent findings were that those with OCD showed elevated CRs during extinction and poor discrimination during extinction recall. Of note is that the two of the three studies that did not report significant group differences or symptom associations during extinction learning (Geller et al., 2019; Tracy et al., 1999) also employed relatively atypical designs compared with the other reviewed articles. Geller et al. (2019) tested dimensional relations within a group of children with OCD and did not include a group of children without OCD symptoms. Tracy et al. (1990) used eyeblink conditioning, which is different in many ways from standard delay fear conditioning designs. The third study which did not find any form of extinction deficit, Giménez et al., 2020, was published as a pilot study and had the smallest N of all the reviewed studies (17 OCD, 13 control). Thus, the amassed evidence for extinction deficits in OCD is relatively consistent when considering the limited number of available studies and the limitations of the studies that did not produce significant extinction differences.
Although significant extinction abnormalities related to OCD were found in most of the reviewed studies, the particular form of the extinction deficit and if the deficit was during initial extinction learning or during extinction recall was not entirely consistent. Specifically, there is a discrepancy between the studies using cue-only conditioning and those using a cue-in-context conditioning design. In terms of the cue-only studies, most analyzed extinction as a function of time (e.g., trial-by-trial or first half vs. second half) and found that those with OCD demonstrated larger CRs to the CS+, compared with control participants, during the later trials of extinction. The normative response reflective of full extinction is for CRs to reach floor for both the CS+ and CS− by the later trials of extinction (e.g., Bouton et al., 2006; Fullana et al., 2018). That several studies showed maintained CRs within the extinction learning session suggests the ability to adaptively regulate behavior is impaired in OCD despite repeated presentations of the CS+ in the absence of the US. However, the four reviewed studies using a cue-in-context design (Fyer et al., 2020; Giménez et al., 2020; McLaughlin et al., 2015; Milad et al., 2013) did not find within-session differences in extinction learning performance between OCD and control groups. Rather, the majority of these studies report group differences in SCRs between-sessions (i.e., during day-two testing). These results support the argument that a strong return of fear after initial extinction (commonly referred to as spontaneous recovery; Pavlov, 1927) is more characteristic of anxiety pathology than initial difficulty extinguishing fear (Bouton et al., 2001; Craske et al., 2011), and align with clinical observation (e.g., Furst & Cooper, 1970; Marks et al., 1969; Pitman, 1993; Rachman et al., 1970) and empirical theory and data (Craske et al., 2008; Kozak et al., 1988) indicating that successful within-session fear reduction does not necessarily correspond to sustained improvement in those with anxiety-related disorders. However, caution is recommended in interpreting extinction recall results given documented methodological inconsistencies across studies, both in this review and in the literature more broadly (Lonsdorf et al., 2019).
A possible explanation for the discrepancy between the cue-only and cue-in-context studies is that the CS-US association is partially dependent on the context in which it is learned, such that stronger CRs to the CS+ emerge in the original conditioning context. The context switch in the cue-in-context design, in which acquisition is conducted in one novel context and extinction in another, potentially weakened the CS-US association (as is seen in occasion setting studies, e.g., Bouton & Swartzentruber, 1986; Myers & Gluck, 1994) to the point that those with OCD could sufficiently extinguish their CR and resemble the control group during this phase.
Neuroimaging results from the two studies using fMRI (Apergis-Schoute et al., 2017; Milad et al., 2013) also potentially provide evidence of an extinction learning deficit in OCD. Both studies found differences in vmPFC activity during extinction between OCD and control groups, although the multiple differences in study and analytic design preclude direct comparisons. Human work based on animal models has proposed the vmPFC as a central region for extinction learning (e.g., Diekhof et al., 2011; Dunsmoor et al., 2019; Phelps et al., 2004). However, the relation between vmPFC and extinction learning is not robust, and a recent meta-analysis did not find the vmPFC to be related to extinction learning across 31 studies. Additionally, the vmPFC has also been implicated as a key point of dysfunction in non-aversive learning processes (Nielen et al., 2009) and as a central contributor to impaired decision making and error-detection in OCD (Nielen et al., 2002; Stern et al., 2011), suggesting that disrupted vmPFC engagement during extinction might reflect a global deficit in the disorder, as opposed to a fear extinction-specific abnormality. Further work is needed to more conclusively determine how vmPFC differences in OCD related to impaired extinction processes.
An important qualifier on the reviewed extinction results is that all the studies that tested extinction learning did so immediately after the acquisition phase, as opposed to testing after a delay. This design is not unique to OCD research. Most human extinction studies, both in clinical and non-clinical samples, use this design (see Duits et al., 2015; Fullana et al., 2018). However, the neural mechanisms underlying immediate extinction learning differ from those underlying delayed extinction learning (Maren, 2014). The clinical implications are different as well, as delayed extinction learning is a more accurate analogue of clinical intervention, which targets an established fear memory given that it typically takes place days, months, or years after initial fear acquisition (e.g., Hollandt et al., 2020).
4.3. Comparing Conditioning Process in OCD to Other Anxiety-Related Disorders
Over decades of conditioning studies of anxiety-related disorders, multiple studies have identified patterns of aversive conditioning deficits that appear to operate trans-diagnostically and represent core pathogenic mechanisms of these disorders. The current review affords the opportunity to compare how conditioning in OCD compares to other anxiety-related disorders. Two comprehensive meta-analyses of behavioral studies in anxiety-related disorders identified increased CS− responding during acquisition and increased CS+ responding during extinction as differentiating anxiety-related disorder groups from control groups (Duits et al., 2015; Lissek et al., 2005). In terms of acquisition, a pattern of elevated CRs to CS+, but no significant differences in CRs to CS− in all but one study (Fyer et al., 2020), distinguishes OCD from the other anxiety-related disorders. It is difficult to explain this discrepancy when considering how OCD differs from other anxiety-related disorders: OCD is perhaps the anxiety-related disorder most easily defined by difficulties with inhibition of unwanted thoughts and behaviors. Therefore, it seems likely CS− deficits would be evident. However, a crucial detail is that data showing poor inhibition in OCD is typically in terms of poor inhibition of compulsions (e.g., Chamberlain et al., 2005; Morein-Zamir et al., 2010). Classical aversive conditioning is assumed to map onto obsession symptoms in OCD more cleanly than compulsions. Therefore, the observed pattern of CS+ responding might not necessarily disagree with prior non-conditioning work. However, this does not explain why OCD would not show CS− acquisition deficits that are apparent in other anxiety-related disorders, and additional work to clarify this discrepancy is needed.
In contrast to acquisition deficits, the CS+ related extinction deficits that differentiated OCD from control groups are consistent with the meta-analyses that also find this pattern in other anxiety-related disorders during initial extinction training, indicating difficulty learning that the CS+ is no longer a signal for the US. Further, the finding in a subset of studies that people with OCD had particular difficulty reducing their CR to the CS+ during the final parts of extinction aligns with several studies of PTSD that obtained a similar pattern of results (for reviews, see Lissek & van Meurs, 2014; Zuj et al., 2016). In terms of extinction recall, the finding that OCD is associated with elevated return of fear compared with control participants resembles findings from some behavioral results, particularly those from studies of PTSD (e.g., Garfinkel et al., 2014; Milad et al., 2008; Rabinak et al., 2017). However, it should be noted that some studies fail to find recall group differences on behavioral measures in PTSD (Milad et al., 2009) or in a trans-diagnostic anxiety-related disorders sample (Marin et al., 2017). Additionally, the noted discrepancy between extinction results from the cue-only and cue-in-context studies align with other studies failing to find initial extinction deficits in PTSD (Garfinkel et al., 2014; Marin et al., 2017; Milad et al., 2008, 2009; Shvil et al., 2014). Prior reviews have suggested that these discrepancies in the PTSD data emerge from cross-study methodological difference and overall weak or inconsistent underlying effects (Lissek & van Meurs, 2014; McGuire, Orr, Essoe, et al., 2016; Zuj et al., 2016), an explanation that could also be applied to the discrepancy observed in the currently reviewed OCD studies.
In terms of functional neuroimaging work involving aversive conditioning and extinction in clinical populations, much of this work has focused on PTSD, likely because conditioning remains dominant in contemporary models of PTSD (e.g., Bowers & Ressler, 2015; Pitman et al., 2012). A recent meta-analysis of seven studies found that PTSD was associated with a robust amygdala response across acquisition and extinction, with aberrant insula and ACC activity related to PTSD during acquisition and vmPFC during extinction (Suarez-Jimenez et al., 2019). Similar results have been found in studies of panic disorder (e.g., Kircher et al., 2013; Schwarzmeier et al., 2019) and generalized anxiety disorder (e.g., Greenberg et al., 2013), although not all anxiety-related disorders are associated with this pattern of neural results, including specific phobia (e.g., Lange et al., 2019) and social anxiety disorder (e.g., Savage et al., 2020). Relatively consistent vmPFC and ACC results from the two neuroimaging studies of conditioning in OCD suggest that, at the neural level, OCD might have more in common with PTSD and the other anxiety-related disorders characterized by vmPFC and ACC dysfunction.
4.4. Design Complexity and Improving Parallels Between Conditioning Techniques and Clinical Reality of OCD
At present, the OCD and conditioning literature is primarily comprised of studies using basic differential acquisition and extinction paradigms that utilize relatively unambiguous and straightforward perceptual cues and contexts. These designs have the advantage of offering more parsimonious interpretation but likely limit the ability to discern more complex and clinically relevant processes as they relate to OCD. Researchers have consistently advocated for the use of more complex conditioning designs to disentangle nuanced or difficult-to-measure emotional learning phenomena (e.g., Beckers et al., 2013; Dunsmoor et al., 2015; Gewirtz & Davis, 2000; Rescorla, 1988), enhanced detection of pathogenic markers (Lissek et al., 2006), and provide further avenues for novel treatment advances (e.g., Craske et al., 2014). In this case, increased complexity refers to experimental manipulations that potentially increase ecological validity through increasing the number and/or abstractness of stimuli, associations between stimuli, or task demands and rules. In the following sections, we consider iterations or elaborations on the basic differential design that were absent from the reviewed literature and how improving design complexity could substantially contribute to conditioning models of OCD, potentially clarify emerging patterns of results, and enhance clinical translation.
4.4.1. Conditioning Through Conceptual and Semantic Routes
Many obsessions are verbal-linguistic in nature (Knapton, 2016), and the complex and frequently abstract or bizarre contents of obsessional thought in OCD are important aspects of the pathology that can differentiate them from non-clinical cognitive intrusions and uncomfortable thoughts (García-Soriano et al., 2011; Rassin et al., 2007). Further, these OCD-related obsessions are distinct from the typically simpler associations that frequently occur in other anxiety-related disorders due to their elaborative and abstracted quality and the level of inductive reasoning involved in their generation. For example, consider a contamination obsession in someone with OCD. The obsession might have started with an initial observation in a bathroom that there were many sensory cues indicating the bathroom is dirty and has germs, such as unwashed floors and aversive smells. However, there is also the known function of a bathroom and the commonly expressed societal view of bathrooms as “dirty places” that contribute to this obsession, which is an abstract concept without a direct sensory correlate. Further, the person has been told that “germs can cause disease” and, even though it is statistically unlikely to contract a disease in a bathroom, catching a disease is the assumed outcome of entering a bathroom and thus a focus of the obsession. The only way for this type of cognition to develop is through abstraction and inductive reasoning, and these processes are indeed identified as related to OCD and linked to a model of obsession development (Liew et al., 2018; O’Connor, 2002; Pélissier & O’Connor, 2002, 2002). Accordingly, understanding this aspect of obsessions is crucial to furthering OCD research and increasing the ecological validity of said research. However, the reviewed studies all use conditioning paradigms that employ perceptual stimuli and test for CRs that are therefore related to more automatic, sensory-level associations and responses. The predominance of perceptual stimuli is not unexpected, as more abstract or conceptual forms of conditioning have received less empirical attention overall (for review, see Dunsmoor & Murphy, 2015), largely owing to the predominance of animal-to-human translations driving conditioning efforts in humans. This represents a notable gap in the literature and an important area for future conditioning work in OCD.
One intuitive explanation for how obsessions spread and become unbearable for people with OCD is through semantic generalization. Exposure hierarchies for OCD typically resemble a personalized list of generalized stimuli (Himle & Franklin, 2009). Given the abstract nature of many obsessions, it follows that many of the associations between these generalized stimuli are conceptual in nature. For example, a person with OCD might report that their primary obsession is about catching germs from dirty surfaces in bathrooms and kitchens. They then might generalize this obsession to people who work in what are considered dirty places (e.g., janitorial staff), places that have lots of people touching multiple surfaces with different body parts (e.g., gyms), and clothing or tools that are used to negate or clean environmental hazards (e.g., face masks, mops) Although there is no perceptual overlap to account for the association between these stimuli, the conceptual association is clear: they are all indirectly related to dirt and germs, some of them quite distally.
Generalization across conceptual pathways is as yet untested in OCD, although it has been successfully implemented in non-clinical samples (e.g., Dunsmoor et al., 2012; Dunsmoor & Murphy, 2014; Vervoort et al., 2014). Further, a recent study by Morey et al. (2020) identified PTSD-specific neural responses to novel stimuli that were part of a conditioned conceptual category (e.g., a participant has not seen a cat yet in a task, but responds fearfully because they were conditioned to fear the mammal category, of which the cat is a member) within fronto-limbic regions commonly associated with perceptual fear generalization. This type of design applied to people with OCD holds promise for more precisely modeling how obsessional content and associations spread on the neural level. Further, the success of the task used by Morey et al. (2020), which uses disorder-aspecific stimuli (e.g., animals and tools) applied to PTSD, suggests that similar results might emerge for those with OCD even without disorder-specific conceptual categories. The use of neutral, disorder-aspecific stimuli in this type of task might help identify if overgeneralization of conceptual associations is a basic mechanism underlying OCD broadly or if it is more circumscribed to the specific type of obsession. Based on the currently reviewed studies, particularly those with neuroimaging findings, we would predict that those with OCD would also overgeneralize fronto-limbic responses across a conceptual category.
4.4.2. Instrumental Conditioning and Approach-Avoidance Conflicts
A fundamental understanding regarding OCD pathology is that compulsions serve to relieve distress related to obsessions, but only temporarily. In turn, this temporary relief reinforces the belief that obsessions are unbearable and that compulsions must be enacted (Meyer, 1966). The obsession-compulsion dynamic fundamentally “holds the disorder together,” and this link is the primary target of EX/RP: if you can disrupt the compulsions, then patients will learn that associated obsessions are tolerable and be less likely to enact compulsions (Foa et al., 2012; Wheaton et al., 2018). Functionally, compulsions serve as a form of avoidance (McGuire et al., 2012), which fits with research that finds avoidance of emotionally salient material is a primary maintenance factor that prolongs many forms of psychopathology (e.g., Borkovec et al., 2004; Hayes et al., 1996; Pittig et al., 2018; Salters-Pedneault et al., 2004). Putting obsessions and compulsions together, we see that they cleanly fit into Mowrer’s influential two-factor theory of anxiety (Mowrer, 1939, 1947, 1951), in which avoidance of a threat cue results in relief that serves as a motivator for future avoidance and prevents opportunities to learn that a feared stimulus is not dangerous. Importantly, this theory links the underlying classical and instrumental (i.e., operant) conditioning processes in a functional framework that explains how the passive-emotional process of classical conditioning exerts its pathogenic influence through the behavior it motivates. Although Mowrer’s model has received criticism and its limitations have been expanded on through empirical testing (Rachman, 1976), the proposed functional relationship between the two conditioning processes remains a compelling foundation for explanatory models of anxiety-related pathology. Further emphasizing the need for instrumental conditioning studies in OCD are neural models of avoidance that implicate frontal and temporal structures central to OCD pathology (e.g., Bravo-Rivera et al., 2015; Rosas-Vidal et al., 2018).
Notably, none of the reviewed conditioning studies included an instrumental conditioning test (or tested compulsions with any other experimental model of avoidance) despite the applicability of Mowrer’s two-factor model and its presence in treatment-focused writings related to OCD (e.g., Franklin & Foa, 2011; Storch et al., 2007). Outside of the aversive conditioning literature, there are a handful of relevant studies of those with OCD or OCD symptoms. These include tests of habitual avoidance (Gillan, Apergis-Schoute, et al., 2014; Gillan, Morein-Zamir, et al., 2014), probabilistic reinforcement learning (Endrass et al., 2011; Nielen et al., 2009; Remijnse et al., 2006; Voon et al., 2015), simple operant conditioning in a non-emotional context (Hassoulas et al., 2014), and self-report studies of subjective, broadly defined avoidance tendencies (e.g., Abramowitz et al., 2009; Ettelt et al., 2008; Summerfeldt et al., 2014). Generally, the patterns of results from these studies indicate that people with OCD or higher levels of OCD-related traits describe themselves as more risk-averse and avoidant, more readily associate avoidance with negative outcomes and increased threat sensitivity, and are more likely to enact avoidance responses than comparison participants in a laboratory setting. However, none of these studies involve truly aversive USs. Therefore, we still know very little about how aversive conditioning contributes to avoidance (and vice versa) and are likely missing key information on how obsessions are associated with compulsions.3
An important next step is formally testing those with OCD on a task that combines classical aversive conditioning and instrumental avoidance. There is a rich history of animal studies that test these processes (e.g., Kirlic et al., 2017; Mackintosh, 1974). Human studies of instrumental avoidance have been less common compared with classical conditioning studies and, until relatively recently, had fallen out of favor in the conditioning field (LeDoux et al., 2017; Pittig et al., 2018). A recent resurgence in this area has resulted in efforts to revitalize the study of avoidance in the context of anxiety-related pathology. These efforts have also resulted in new experimental paradigms, or modifications of past paradigms, that hold great promise for the study of OCD. Perhaps the most relevant are avoidance tasks that require an active response to remove or escape the aversive outcome (analogous to enacting a compulsion) and tasks in which avoidance has an associated cost (e.g., Hunt et al., 2019; Pittig & Scherbaum, 2019; Rattel et al., 2017). Cost is a particularly important parameter to model in experimental studies of OCD, as part of the clinical definition of compulsions is that these behaviors are repeated excessively or otherwise interfere with a person’s life, thus denying more valued activity. For example, some people with OCD have very strong checking obsessions and compulsions report social and romantic discord due to others accommodating the time-intensive and frequently inconvenient checking behavior of the person with OCD (e.g., Boeding et al., 2013). In other words, the “cost” of avoidant behavior is the valued outcome of relational stability. Conditioning tasks that test these situations, termed approach-avoidance conflicts or mixed outcome designs, typically involve an inherently salient reward that, when removed or lost, serves as the cost of an avoidance response and can potentially buffer against avoidance of a feared stimulus (Pittig et al., 2014, 2018). Examples of rewards used in these tasks are monetary compensation or winning a specific task and receiving points (for review, see Pittig et al., 2018).
4.4.3. Inhibitory Learning During Acquisition and Extinction
Although aversive conditioning is commonly used as an explanatory model for excitatory responding that results in excessive negative emotionality (e.g., a strong fear response in anxiety-related disorders), understanding how people learn to inhibit their conditioned responses has clear scientific and clinical relevance. Inhibitory learning is of particular interest to the study of OCD because of clinical conceptualizations of the disorder centrally involving failures to inhibit obsessions and compulsions, as well as consistent experimental evidence for neural abnormalities in inhibitory regions (e.g., Thorsen et al., 2018). As with excitatory learning, inhibitory learning plays a central role in both acquisition and extinction of conditioned associations. Below we discuss each separately in the context of future studies of OCD.
During the initial acquisition of an aversive association, excitatory and inhibitory information and processes simultaneously contribute to the expressed CR (e.g., Fullana et al., 2016; Krabbe et al., 2018). Thus, the CR can be seen as the result of competition between excitatory and inhibitory influences, with the stronger of the two dictating the behavioral expression for a particular CS. Although the reviewed studies suggest that CS+, but not CS−, responding during acquisition differentiated OCD groups from the control group, this does not necessarily correspond to impaired inhibitory learning in OCD. The observed CR to a CS+ or CS− is comprised of some combination of excitatory and inhibitory activity, but the exact contributions of each are not identifiable using standard differential conditioning designs (Jovanovic et al., 2005; Myers & Davis, 2004). For example, a relatively decreased CR to the CS− could be equally related to a decrease in excitatory activation or an increase in inhibitory activation. Similarly, a strong CR to a CS+ could equally be the result of a complete lack of inhibitory activation or a maximal excitatory response that has been tampered down by a moderate inhibitory response.
An elegant solution to the issue of entangled excitatory and inhibitory processes is the conditioned inhibition paradigm (Rescorla, 1969; Rescorla & Holland, 1977). This paradigm is a modification of the differential conditioning design in which some trials present the previously conditioned CS+ simultaneously alongside a novel stimulus and without US reinforcement. This novel stimulus is then combined with the CS− and other novel stimuli to measure inhibitory learning, which is operationalized as a lack of CR on these later trials. Researchers have used conditioned inhibition paradigms to measure PTSD-control differences in inhibitory responding (e.g., Jovanovic et al., 2010; Jovanovic & Norrholm, 2011; Sijbrandij et al., 2013), and this paradigm would also be a logical next step for OCD conditioning work. Despite acquisition in OCD being somewhat biased towards CS+ reactivity, we would expect that conditioned inhibition during acquisition would also be impaired in OCD relative to comparison participants and reflect the broader inhibition learning deficits – both emotional and non-emotional – that are commonly seen in the disorder (e.g., Berlin et al., 2015; Enright & Beech, 1993; Kampman et al., 2002; van Velzen et al., 2014).
Inhibitory learning during extinction is crucial to understanding the mechanisms and conditions through which a CR can decrease and eventually abate. Modern understanding of the extinction process is that through repeated presentation of the CS+ in the absence of the US, two competing memory traces are formed: the original CS+−US association and a new inhibitory memory of the CS+ without the US (Bouton, 1993, 2004). A corollary to this extinction model is that CR reductions are not necessarily solely driven by repeated exposure (habituation) to the CS+ that results in a weakening of the CS+−US association and instead also involve promotion of the competing inhibitory memory. Consequently, exposure therapy’s effectiveness is not exclusively dependent on successful habituation, as initially proposed (Foa & Kozak, 1986), but is also related to the strength of new inhibitory memories and experiences (Craske et al., 2008, 2014, 2018; Tolin, 2019). Modern research on extinction learning and exposure therapy has started to center on how to harness inhibitory learning models most effectively during treatment, with OCD discussed as one disorder that would benefit from this type of work (e.g., Arch & Abramowitz, 2015; Craske et al., 2008; Jacoby & Abramowitz, 2016; Krompinger et al., 2019). Findings of OCD-related extinction deficits in the reviewed studies, as well as the need to improve upon the limitations of EX/RP, suggest that this would be a fruitful avenue of future research and a natural next step for conditioning investigations of OCD.
A potential template for future empirical studies can be found in the clinical techniques developed in line with the inhibitory learning model and based on basic science work (Dunsmoor et al., 2015), but still need validation within OCD samples. Craske et al. (2014) detail eight techniques with strong conditioning foundations, and a review by Jacoby and Abramowitz (2016) specifies four techniques that hold great promise for treatment of OCD in particular: expectancy violation (maximizing the discrepancy between the expected and actual aversive outcome), combining multiple fear cues/deepened extinction (increasing inhibitory learning by extinguishing two or more CS+s during extinction), maximizing contextual variability (presenting extinction in multiple contexts to promote durable and generalized CR reductions), and expanding the inter-session interval (increasing the duration between extinction trials). All these techniques have corresponding laboratory-based conditioning protocols and are ideal next steps for OCD conditioning research that builds off the reviewed studies’ findings.
4.4.4. Adjuncts to Exposure Therapy and Improved Conditioning Models of OCD
Exposure techniques for OCD can be enhanced with therapeutic adjuncts from outside of the conditioning toolbox to improve effectiveness. However, the interaction between these adjunct interventions and conditioning techniques and processes is not well-studied in OCD, and results in other anxiety-related disorders or pre-clinical results are ambiguous. Improved conditioning methodology that more precisely tests clinically relevant processes in OCD could be valuable for future research on these adjunct interventions.
For example, pharmacological enhancement of extinction has been a goal for clinical researchers for decades (Fitzgerald et al., 2014). D-cycloserine (DCS), a N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor partial agonist, has been put forward as a potential extinction enhancer and tested in multiple clinical trials for OCD and other anxiety-related disorders. Meta-analytic results indicate that DCS has a small-to-null faciliatory effect on exposure treatment in anxiety-related disorders, with some of the smallest observed effects seen in studies of OCD (e.g., Bürkner et al., 2017; Mataix-Cols et al., 2017; McGuire et al., 2016). Other meta-analyses suggest that DCS is a viable adjunct for exposure under certain timing and dose parameters (e.g., Rosenfield et al., 2019; Xia et al., 2015). However, these meta-analyses are somewhat at odds with meta-analytic findings that DCS improves fear extinction in experimental animal and human studies (Norberg et al., 2008), and with some moderators even showing opposite effects in the clinical vs. experimental literature (e.g., fewer DCS doses enhanced extinction in experimental studies, Norberg et al., 2008, but more DCS doses enhanced exposure in clinical studies, Rosenfield et al., 2019). The questions that arise from these conflicting meta-analytic findings suggest that increased insight into the mechanisms underlying DCS and its relation to the processes underlying exposure therapy is needed. In terms of behavioral adjuncts to exposure therapy, one intriguing option is improving deliberate emotion regulation, such as reappraising or suppressing an emotional response or impetus. Evidence for emotion regulation strategies enhancing extinction (Delgado et al., 2008; Hennings et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2021) or exposure therapy (Renna et al., 2017; Wisco et al., 2013) in non-clinical and clinical populations is mixed. Accordingly, conditioning tasks that more closely align with the clinical reality of OCD and might provide more precise and ecologically valid outcomes could be valuable for future studies of DCS, emotion regulation, and other candidate pharmacological and behavioral adjuncts to exposure.
4.5. Clinical Considerations in Conditioning Studies of OCD: Assessment, Classification, and Medication
4.5.1. Assessment and Classification
Most of the reviewed conditioning studies employ the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) to assign a categorical OCD diagnosis. Those studies that contain dimensional symptom measures either use those measures to form analog groups that are analyzed categorically or do not assess all participants (e.g., those designated as psychiatric controls) with these measures. Categorical diagnostic systems, including the DSM, have increasingly received intense criticism (Kotov et al., 2017; Krueger et al., 2018; Watson, 2005). As it pertains to OCD, evidence broadly suggests that OCD does not conform to the categorical DSM structure (Kotov et al., 2011; Pinto et al., 2008; Raines et al., 2015), and the DSM category for OCD is viewed as inflexible for clinical and research use (Hollander et al., 2007). Further, heterogeneity within the disorder category is problematically high (Abramowitz & Jacoby, 2015; Lochner & Stein, 2003; McKay et al., 2004) and results in the grouping together of people who drastically differ from each other in terms of the themes of their obsessions and compulsions (Baer, 1994; Leckman et al., 1997; Summerfeldt et al., 1999, 2014). Conversely, dimensional trait systems are an appropriate fit for reported OCD symptoms and psychometrically outperform categorical diagnosis of OCD while better accounting for symptom heterogeneity (see Kotov et al., 2007, 2010, 2015; Watson, 2005).
Adapting a dimensional assessment and classification approach in future conditioning studies of OCD could yield numerous benefits. At a statistical level, dimensional indices of OCD naturally yield greater variance than categorical indices. Accordingly, testing dimensional indices of OCD as a primary predictor could result in increased precision to detect subtle conditioning abnormalities related to the pathology. The use of multivariate techniques to identify latent dimensions that cut across DSM-defined OCD criteria (such as empirically identified OCD symptom dimensions or higher-order internalizing traits, e.g., Faure & Forbes, 2021) might also improve detection of relevant conditioning abnormalities related to the pathology.
Dimensional approaches also help address psychiatric comorbidities that obscure a clearer picture of psychopathology and its relations (e.g., Kim & Eaton, 2015; Kotov et al., 2017). Comorbidity, a natural consequence of a categorical classification system, creates difficulties in reliably assigning etiological or pathophysiological processes to specific categories (e.g., Krueger & Markon, 2006). In the current review, almost all the tested clinical groups contained participants who were also diagnosed with other anxiety-related disorders or mood disorders. Thus, the possibility remains that the patterns of conditioning deficits are not necessarily related to “pure” OCD but rather reflect an amalgamation of pathogenic processes in both OCD and commonly comorbid disorders. Alternatively, it is possible that the comorbidities created additional noise in an OCD-specific signal and that a more consistent pattern of conditioning deficits in OCD would emerge if comorbid presentations were excluded from tested samples. Adapting a dimensional approach could help address both of these possibilities, as each potential source of conditioning-related individual variation (e.g., OCD symptoms and traits, mood disorder symptoms and traits, etc.) could be included and tested in a statistical model.
To facilitate a more dimensional approach to OCD assessment, future studies might consider use of the Dimensional Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (DY-BOCS; Rosario-Campos et al., 2006). The DY-BOCS provides fine-grained OCD symptom severity subscales that are not typically derived from the standard adult or child Y-BOCS (Goodman et al., 1989; Goodman et al., 1991) that was used in many of the reviewed studies (see Table 1). In addition to a primary interview measure of OCD symptom severity, we recommend employing an additional self-report measure when possible (e.g., Abramowitz et al., 2010, Foa et al., 2002); primarily because of documented differences in reported symptoms based on the measurement technique (Federici et al., 2010). Additionally, some OCD self-report measures are able to cover a broader range of content than interviews.
4.5.2. Medications
In the current review, the majority of studies tested OCD groups that contained participants reporting current use of psychotropic medication (see Table 1). The most frequently reported medication was the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) class of antidepressants that are frequently prescribed as a first-line pharmaceutical treatment for anxiety-related disorders and show acceptable efficacy (Soomro et al., 2008). Animal work finds that acute serotonergic challenge reliably attenuates conditioned fear acquisition and extinction (for review, see Inoue et al., 2011). Human studies are less conclusive: one study found SSRI treatment facilitates extinction (Bui et al., 2013), whereas another found that use of SSRIs, along with other medications, weakened but did not eliminate significant anxiety patient-control differences in fear acquisition (Otto et al., 2014). Other common psychotropic medications used by participants in the reviewed studies, such as sedatives and beta-blockers, have also been shown to affect human fear expression (e.g., Grillon et al., 2004, 2006). Specific to OCD, there is evidence that medication usage in studies of OCD patients confounds efforts to identify reliable biomarkers of the disorder (for review, see Fullana et al., 2020); and that machine learning algorithms applied to neural data reliably distinguished between medicated and unmedicated patients with OCD at a higher rate than OCD patient vs. control (see Heuvel et al., 2020). Taken together, there is a clear need to systematically investigate medication usage in OCD within the context of conditioning studies to determine the degree and form of confounding effect. That said, we contend that restricting study participation to those with OCD who are medication-free is not the answer. In the interest of generalizability and translation, future studies would benefit from studying conditioning processes within the clinical reality of OCD, and that clinical reality frequently involves psychotropic medication.
4.6. Conclusion
Our review of conditioning studies of OCD has revealed that a small but promising body of research supports a conditioning perspective of the disorder, especially regarding extinction deficits. Current priorities in the treatment research suggest that the next wave of conditioning work in OCD would benefit from building on basic acquisition and extinction paradigms to address more experimentally complex but clinically relevant questions. We hope that with future conditioning studies, translational scientists will have a robust and detailed foundation of data on which to build the next wave of exposure treatments, as has been done in the past in regard to other anxiety-related disorders. We also anticipate that the addition of new studies to the growing OCD conditioning literature will be conducive to an eventual quantitative synthesis (e.g., meta-analysis) in the future. Although the bulk of the scientific work has likely yet to be done, we view the field as well-positioned to make a scientifically impactful and clinically meaningful difference for those with OCD.
Highlights.
Classical conditioning models of OCD inform gold-standard exposure interventions.
First systematic review of classical fear conditioning studies in OCD.
Twelve identified studies yielded mixed evidence for impaired acquisition in OCD, but more consistent evidence for extinction deficits.
Next steps and recommendations for ecologically-valid laboratory tasks and individual difference assessments to facilitate translational efforts are discussed.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by NIH R01 MH122387, NIH R00 MH106719, and NSF CAREER Award 1844792 to JED.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Declaration of Interests
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
The reported mean symptom severity for the OCD group from McLaughlin et al. (2015) is roughly comparable with reviewed studies testing those with current OCD diagnoses only, although it should be noted that the standard deviation is larger and likely reflects less-symptomatic individuals who might not meet full OCD criteria. That said, given that most participants in the OCD group are likely reporting significant symptoms, McLaughlin et al. (2015) appears to be more accurately described as testing a clinical, rather than an analogue, OCD group.
The use of disgust-conditioning differentiates Armstrong and Olatunji (2017) from the other reviewed studies, which primarily test fear-conditioning. Conditioning differences between the two emotions have been directly compared and were found to share many of the same neural circuitry (Klucken et al., 2012), and affective responses to fear vs. disgust stimuli were mostly similar in an OCD sample (Schienle et al., 2005). Thus, we contend that disgust- and fear-conditioning sufficiently overlap for the purposes of the current review.
It should also be noted that differing views on compulsions in OCD exist, such as compulsions being conceptualized as the manifestation of broad and excessive habit formation as opposed to behaviors that are enacted with a goal of avoidance (Gillan & Robbins, 2014). We acknowledge that multiple processes are likely contributing to the behavioral output of compulsions, but that at the present we cannot directly compare habit models of avoidance to conditioning models of avoidance in OCD due to the lack of studies of the latter. A recent review on this subject provides a more detailed and neurobiologically informed discussion of this issue (Geramita et al., 2020), and other reviews delineate and compare the forms and stages of avoidance more broadly (e.g., LeDoux & Daw, 2018).
References
*included in systematic review.
- Abramowitz JS (1996). Variants of exposure and response prevention in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Behavior Therapy, 27, 583–600. [Google Scholar]
- Abramowitz JS, Deacon BJ, Olatunji BO, Wheaton MG, Berman NC, Losardo D, Timpano KR, McGrath PB, Riemann BC, & Adams T (2010). Assessment of obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions: Development and evaluation of the Dimensional Obsessive-Compulsive Scale. Psychological Assessment, 22, 180. 10.1037/a0018260 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abramowitz JS, Fabricant LE, Taylor S, Deacon BJ, McKay D, & Storch EA (2014). The relevance of analogue studies for understanding obsessions and compulsions. Clinical Psychology Review, 34, 206–217. 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abramowitz JS, & Jacoby RJ (2015). Obsessive-compulsive and related disorders: A critical review of the new diagnostic class. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 165–186. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153713 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abramowitz JS, Lackey GR, & Wheaton MG (2009). Obsessive–compulsive symptoms: The contribution of obsessional beliefs and experiential avoidance. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 160–166. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abramowitz JS, Taylor S, & McKay D (2007). Psychological theories of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Handbook of Child and Adolescent Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, 109–129. [Google Scholar]
- Abramowitz JS, Taylor S, McKay D, & Deacon BJ (2011). Animal models of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 69, e29–e30. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmari SE, & Dougherty DD (2015). Dissecting OCD circuits: From animal models to targeted treatments. Depression and Anxiety, 32, 550–562. 10.1002/da.22367 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander GE, DeLong MR, & Strick PL (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9, 357–381. 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Fifth Edition). American Psychiatric Association. http://psychiatryonline.org/doi/book/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 [Google Scholar]
- *.Apergis-Schoute AM, Gillan CM, Fineberg NA, Fernandez-Egea E, Sahakian BJ, & Robbins TW (2017). Neural basis of impaired safety signaling in obsessive compulsive disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 3216–3221. 10.1073/pnas.1609194114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arch JJ, & Abramowitz JS (2015). Exposure therapy for obsessive–compulsive disorder: An optimizing inhibitory learning approach. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 6, 174–182. 10.1016/j.jocrd.2014.12.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Armstrong T, & Olatunji BO (2017). Pavlovian disgust conditioning as a model for contamination-based OCD: Evidence from an analogue study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 93, 78–87. 10.1016/j.brat.2017.03.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baer L (1994). Factor analysis of symptom subtypes of obsessive compulsive disorder and their relation to personality and tic disorders. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 55 Suppl, 18–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckers T, Krypotos A-M, Boddez Y, Effting M, & Kindt M (2013). What’s wrong with fear conditioning? Biological Psychology, 92, 90–96. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlin HA, Schulz KP, Zhang S, Turetzky R, Rosenthal D, & Goodman W (2015). Neural correlates of emotional response inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A preliminary study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 234, 259–264. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.09.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blechert J, Michael T, Williams SL, Purkis HM, & Wilhelm FH (2008). When two paradigms meet: Does evaluative learning extinguish in differential fear conditioning? Learning and Motivation, 39, 58–70. 10.1016/j.lmot.2007.03.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Boeding SE, Paprocki CM, Baucom DH, Abramowitz JS, Wheaton MG, Fabricant LE, & Fischer MS (2013). Let me check that for you: Symptom accommodation in romantic partners of adults with Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 316–322. 10.1016/j.brat.2013.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borkovec TD, Alcaine OM, & Behar E (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and generalized anxiety disorder. In Heimberg RG, Turk CL, & Mennin DS (Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice. (pp. 77–108). Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Boulougouris V, Chamberlain SR, & Robbins TW (2009). Cross-species models of OCD spectrum disorders. Psychiatry Research, 170, 15–21. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80–99. 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME (2002). Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 976–986. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01546-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 485–494. 10.1101/lm.78804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Mineka S, & Barlow DH (2001). A modern learning theory perspective on the etiology of panic disorder. Psychological Review, 108, 4–32. 10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, & Swartzentruber D (1986). Analysis of the associative and occasion-setting properties of contexts participating in a Pavlovian discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 333–350. 10.1037/0097-7403.12.4.333 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Westbrook RF, Corcoran KA, & Maren S (2006). Contextual and temporal modulation of extinction: Behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 352–360. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowers ME, & Ressler KJ (2015). An overview of translationally informed treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder: animal models of Pavlovian fear conditioning to human clinical trials. Biological Psychiatry, 78, E15–E27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bürkner P-C, Bittner N, Holling H, & Buhlmann U (2017). D-cycloserine augmentation of behavior therapy for anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorders: A meta-analysis. PLOS ONE, 12, e0173660. 10.1371/journal.pone.0173660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brady RE, Adams TG, & Lohr JM (2010). Disgust in contamination-based obsessive–compulsive disorder: A review and model. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 10, 1295–1305. 10.1586/ern.10.46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bravo-Rivera C, Roman-Ortiz C, Montesinos-Cartagena M, & Quirk GJ (2015). Persistent active avoidance correlates with activity in prelimbic cortex and ventral striatum. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9. 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breiter HC, & Rauch SL (1996). Functional MRI and the study of OCD: From symptom provocation to cognitive-behavioral probes of cortico-striatal systems and the amygdala. NeuroImage, 4, S127–S138. 10.1006/nimg.1996.0063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bui E, Orr SP, Jacoby RJ, Keshaviah A, LeBlanc NJ, Milad MR, Pollack MH, & Simon NM (2013). Two weeks of pretreatment with escitalopram facilitates extinction learning in healthy individuals. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 28, 447–456. 10.1002/hup.2330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavedini P, Gorini A, & Bellodi L (2006). Understanding obsessive–compulsive disorder: Focus on decision making. Neuropsychology Review, 16, 3–15. 10.1007/s11065-006-9001-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, & Sahakian BJ (2005). The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: The importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 399–419. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cisler JM, Olatunji BO, & Lohr JM (2009). Disgust, fear, and the anxiety disorders: A critical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 34–46. 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.09.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook M, Mineka S, Wolkenstein B, & Laitsch K (1985). Observational conditioning of snake fear in unrelated rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 591–610. 10.1037/0021-843X.94.4.591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craske MG, Hermans D, & Vervliet B (2018). State-of-the-art and future directions for extinction as a translational model for fear and anxiety. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1742), 20170025. 10.1098/rstb.2017.0025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craske MG, Kircanski K, Zelikowsky M, Mystkowski J, Chowdhury N, & Baker A (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 5–27. 10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craske MG, Rauch SL, Ursano R, Prenoveau J, Pine DS, & Zinbarg RE (2011). What Is an Anxiety Disorder? Depression and Anxiety, 9, 369–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craske MG, Treanor M, Conway CC, Zbozinek T, & Vervliet B (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 58, 10–23. 10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear-potentiated startle: Implications for animal models of anxiety. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 13, 35–41. 10.1016/0165-6147(92)90014-W [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer J, Thomas S, & Baeyens F (2001). Association learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 853–869. 10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.853 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Casale A, Kotzalidis GD, Rapinesi C, Serata D, Ambrosi E, Simonetti A, Pompili M, Ferracuti S, Tatarelli R, & Girardi P (2011). Functional neuroimaging in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychobiology, 64, 61–85. 10.1159/000325223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delgado MR, Nearing KI, LeDoux JE, & Phelps EA (2008). Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction. Neuron, 59, 829–838. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diekhof EK, Geier K, Falkai P, & Gruber O (2011). Fear is only as deep as the mind allows: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on the regulation of negative affect. NeuroImage, 58, 275–285. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duits P, Cath DC, Lissek S, Hox JJ, Hamm AO, Engelhard IM, van den Hout MA, & Baas JMP (2015). Updated meta-analysis of classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 32, 239–253. 10.1002/da.22353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Kroes MC, Li J, Daw ND, Simpson HB, & Phelps EA (2019). Role of human ventromedial prefrontal cortex in learning and recall of enhanced extinction. Journal of Neuroscience, 39, 3264–3276. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2713-18.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Martin A, & LaBar KS (2012). Role of conceptual knowledge in learning and retention of conditioned fear. Biological Psychology, 89, 300–305. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, & Murphy GL (2014). Stimulus typicality determines how broadly fear is generalized. Psychological Science, 25, 1816–1821. 10.1177/0956797614535401 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, & Murphy GL (2015). Categories, concepts, and conditioning: How humans generalize fear. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 73–77. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Niv Y, Daw N, & Phelps EA (2015). Rethinking extinction. Neuron, 88, 47–63. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisen JL, Sibrava NJ, Boisseau CL, Mancebo MC, Stout RL, Pinto A, & Rasmussen SA (2013). Five-year course of obsessive-compulsive disorder: Predictors of remission and relapse. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 74, 233–239. 10.4088/JCP.12m07657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Endrass T, Kloft L, Kaufmann C, & Kathmann N (2011). Approach and avoidance learning in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 166–172. 10.1002/da.20772 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enright SJ, & Beech AR (1993). Reduced cognitive inhibition in obsessive—Compulsive disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32, 67–74. 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1993.tb01028.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etkin, M. D., & Wager, Ph. D. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: A meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1476–1488. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ettelt S, Grabe HJ, Ruhrmann S, Buhtz F, Hochrein A, Kraft S, Pukrop R, Klosterkötter J, Falkai P, Maier W, John U, Freyberger HJ, & Wagner M (2008). Harm avoidance in subjects with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their families. Journal of Affective Disorders, 107, 265–269. 10.1016/j.jad.2007.08.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faure K, & Forbes MK (2021). Clarifying the placement of obsessive-compulsive disorder in the empirical structure of psychopathology. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 10.1007/s10862-021-09868-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Federici A, Summerfeldt LJ, Harrington JL, McCabe RE, Purdon CL, Rowa K, & Antony MM (2010). Consistency between self-report and clinician-administered versions of the Yale-Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 729–733. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher PL, & Wells A (2005). How effective are cognitive and behavioral treatments for obsessive–compulsive disorder? A clinical significance analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1543–1558. 10.1016/j.brat.2004.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald PJ, Seemann JR, & Maren S (2014). Can fear extinction be enhanced? A review of pharmacological and behavioral findings. Brain Research Bulletin, 105, 46–60. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2013.12.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, Huppert JD, Leiberg S, Langner R, Kichic R, Hajcak G, & Salkovskis PM (2002). The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: Development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14, 485–495. 10.1037//1040-3590.14.4.485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, & Kozak MJ (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20–35. 10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, & McLean CP (2016). The efficacy of exposure therapy for anxiety-related disorders and its underlying mechanisms: The case of OCD and PTSD. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12, 1–28. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, Yadin E, & Lichner TK (2012). Exposure and response (ritual) prevention for obsessive compulsive disorder: Therapist guide. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Franklin ME, & Foa EB (2011). Treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 229–243. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fullana MA, Abramovitch A, Via E, López-Sola C, Goldberg X, Reina N, Fortea L, Solanes A, Buckley MJ, Ramella-Cravaro V, Carvalho AF, Tortella-Feliu M, Vieta E, Soriano-Mas C, Lázaro L, Stein DJ, Fernández de la Cruz L, Mataix-Cols D, & Radua J (2020). Diagnostic biomarkers for obsessive-compulsive disorder: A reasonable quest or ignis fatuus? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 118, 504–513. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fullana MA, Albajes-Eizagirre A, Soriano-Mas C, Vervliet B, Cardoner N, Benet O, Radua J, & Harrison BJ (2018). Fear extinction in the human brain: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies in healthy participants. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 88, 16–25. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fullana MA, Harrison BJ, Soriano-Mas C, Vervliet B, Cardoner N, Àvila-Parcet A, & Radua J (2016). Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: An updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Molecular Psychiatry, 21, 500–508. 10.1038/mp.2015.88 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fullana MA, Mataix-Cols D, Caspi A, Harrington H, Grisham JR, Moffitt TE, & Poulton R (2009). Obsessions and compulsions in the community: Prevalence, interference, help-seeking, developmental stability, and co-occurring psychiatric conditions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 329–336. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08071006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furst JB, & Cooper A (1970). Failure of systematic desensitization in 2 cases of obsessive-compulsive neurosis marked by fears of insecticide. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8, 203–206. 10.1016/0005-7967(70)90091-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Fyer AJ, Schneier FR, Simpson HB, Choo TH, Tacopina S, Kimeldorf MB, Steinglass JE, Wall M, & Walsh BT (2020). Heterogeneity in fear processing across and within anxiety, eating, and compulsive disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders, 275, 329–338. 10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- García-Soriano G, Belloch A, Morillo C, & Clark DA (2011). Symptom dimensions in obsessive–compulsive disorder: From normal cognitive intrusions to clinical obsessions. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 474–482. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.11.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garfinkel SN, Abelson JL, King AP, Sripada RK, Wang X, Gaines LM, & Liberzon I (2014). Impaired contextual modulation of memories in PTSD: An fMRI and psychophysiological study of extinction retention and fear renewal. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(40), 13435–13443. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4287-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Geller DA, McGuire JF, Orr SP, Pine DS, Britton JC, Small BJ, Murphy TK, Wilhelm S, & Storch EA (2017). Fear conditioning and extinction in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 29, 17–26. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Geller DA, McGuire JF, Orr SP, Small BJ, Murphy TK, Trainor K, Porth R, Wilhelm S, & Storch EA (2019). Fear extinction learning as a predictor of response to cognitive behavioral therapy for pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 64, 1–8. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2019.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geramita MA, Yttri EA, & Ahmari SE (2020). The two-step task, avoidance, and OCD. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 98, 1007–1019. 10.1002/jnr.24594 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gewirtz JC, & Davis M (2000). Using Pavlovian higher-order conditioning paradigms to investigate the neural substrates of emotional learning and memory. Learning & Memory, 7, 257–266. 10.1101/lm.35200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillan CM, Apergis-Schoute AM, Morein-Zamir S, Urcelay GP, Sule A, Fineberg NA, Sahakian BJ, & Robbins TW (2014). Functional neuroimaging of avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 284–293. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040525 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillan CM, Morein-Zamir S, Urcelay GP, Sule A, Voon V, Apergis-Schoute AM, Fineberg NA, Sahakian BJ, & Robbins TW (2014). Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 631–638. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillan CM, & Robbins TW (2014). Goal-directed learning and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1655), 20130475. 10.1098/rstb.2013.0475 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Giménez M, Cano M, Martínez-Zalacaín I, Real E, Alonso P, Segalàs C, Munuera J, Kegeles LS, Weinstein JJ, Xu X, Menchón JM, Cardoner N, Soriano-Mas C, & Fullana MA (2020). Is glutamate associated with fear extinction and cognitive behavior therapy outcome in OCD? A pilot study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 270, 1003–1014. 10.1007/s00406-019-01056-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman WK, Price LH, Rasmussen SA, Mazure C, Fleischmann RL, Hill CL, Heninger GR, & Charney DS (1989). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: I. Development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 1006–1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman W, Price L, Rasmussen S, Riddle M, & Rapoport J (1991). Children’s Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (CY-BOCS). New Haven, Connecticut: Clinical Neuroscience Unit. [Google Scholar]
- Graybiel AM, & Rauch SL (2000). Toward a neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuron, 28, 343–347. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00113-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg T, Carlson JM, Cha J, Hajcak G, & Mujica-Parodi LR (2013). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex reactivity is altered in generalized anxiety disorder during fear generalization. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 242–250. 10.1002/da.22016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hassoulas A, McHugh L, & Reed P (2014). Avoidance and behavioural flexibility in obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28, 148–153. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Wilson KG, Gifford EV, Follette VM, & Strosahl K (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1152–1168. 10.1037/0022-006X.64.6.1152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hennings AC, Bibb SA, Lewis-Peacock JA, & Dunsmoor JE (2021). Thought suppression inhibits the generalization of fear extinction. Behavioural Brain Research, 398, 112931. 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112931 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermans D, Baeyens F, & Vervliet B (2013). Generalization of acquired emotional responses. Handbook of Cognition and Emotion, 117–134. [Google Scholar]
- van den Heuvel OA, Boedhoe PSW, Bertolin S, Bruin WB, Francks C, Ivanov I, Jahanshad N, Kong X-Z, Kwon JS, O’Neill J, Paus T, Patel Y, Piras F, Schmaal L, Soriano-Mas C, Spalletta G, van Wingen GA, Yun J-Y, Vriend C, … Stein DJ (2020). An overview of the first 5 years of the ENIGMA obsessive–compulsive disorder working group: The power of worldwide collaboration. Human Brain Mapping. 10.1002/hbm.24972 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hezel DM, & McNally RJ (2016). A theoretical review of cognitive biases and deficits in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Biological Psychology, 121, 221–232. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Himle MB, & Franklin ME (2009). The more you do it, the easier it gets: Exposure and response prevention for OCD. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16, 29–39. 10.1016/j.cbpra.2008.03.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hollander E, Kim S, Khanna S, & Pallanti S (2007). Obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders: Diagnostic and dimensional issues. CNS Spectrums, 12, 5–13. 10.1017/S1092852900002467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollandt M, Wroblewski A, Yang Y, Ridderbusch IC, Kircher T, Hamm AO, Straube B, & Richter J (2020). Facilitating translational science in anxiety disorders by adjusting extinction training in the laboratory to exposure-based therapy procedures. Translational Psychiatry, 10, 1–10. 10.1038/s41398-020-0786-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homan P, Levy I, Feltham E, Gordon C, Hu J, Li J, Pietrzak RH, Southwick S, Krystal JH, Harpaz-Rotem I, & Schiller D (2019). Neural computations of threat in the aftermath of combat trauma. Nature Neuroscience, 22, 470–476. 10.1038/s41593-018-0315-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt C, Cooper SE, Hartnell MP, & Lissek S (2019). Anxiety sensitivity and intolerance of uncertainty facilitate associations between generalized Pavlovian fear and maladaptive avoidance decisions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 128, 315–326. 10.1037/abn0000422 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huppert JD, Simpson HB, Nissenson KJ, Liebowitz MR, & Foa EB (2009). Quality of life and functional impairment in obsessive–compulsive disorder: A comparison of patients with and without comorbidity, patients in remission, and healthy controls. Depression and Anxiety, 26, 39–45. 10.1002/da.20506 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inoue T, Kitaichi Y, & Koyama T (2011). SSRIs and conditioned fear. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35, 1810–1819. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacoby RJ, & Abramowitz JS (2016). Inhibitory learning approaches to exposure therapy: A critical review and translation to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 49, 28–40. 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnco C, McGuire JF, Roper T, & Storch EA (2020). A meta-analysis of dropout rates from exposure with response prevention and pharmacological treatment for youth with obsessive compulsive disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 37, 407–417. 10.1002/da.22978 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jovanovic T, Keyes M, Fiallos A, Myers KM, Davis M, & Duncan EJ (2005). Fear potentiation and fear inhibition in a human fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1559–1564. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jovanovic T, & Norrholm SD (2011). Neural mechanisms of impaired fear inhibition in posttraumatic stress disorder. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 44. 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jovanovic T, Norrholm SD, Blanding NQ, Davis M, Duncan E, Bradley B, & Ressler KJ (2010). Impaired fear inhibition is a biomarker of PTSD but not depression. Depression and Anxiety, 27, 244–251. 10.1002/da.20663 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jovanovic T, Nylocks KM, Gamwell KL, Smith A, Davis TA, Norrholm SD, & Bradley B (2014). Development of fear acquisition and extinction in children: Effects of age and anxiety. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 113, 135–142. 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kampman M, Keijsers GPJ, Verbraak MJPM, Näring G, & Hoogduin CAL (2002). The emotional Stroop: A comparison of panic disorder patients, obsessive–compulsive patients, and normal controls, in two experiments. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 425–441. 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00127-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim H, & Eaton NR (2015). The hierarchical structure of common mental disorders: Connecting multiple levels of comorbidity, bifactor models, and predictive validity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124, 1064–1078. 10.1037/abn0000113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kircher T, Arolt V, Jansen A, Pyka M, Reinhardt I, Kellermann T, Konrad C, Lueken U, Gloster AT, Gerlach AL, Ströhle A, Wittmann A, Pfleiderer B, Wittchen H-U, & Straube B (2013). Effect of cognitive-behavioral therapy on neural correlates of fear conditioning in panic disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 73, 93–101. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirlic N, Young J, & Aupperle RL (2017). Animal to human translational paradigms relevant for approach avoidance conflict decision making. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 96, 14–29. 10.1016/j.brat.2017.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klucken T, Schweckendiek J, Koppe G, Merz CJ, Kagerer S, Walter B, Sammer G, Vaitl D, & Stark R (2012). Neural correlates of disgust- and fear-conditioned responses. Neuroscience, 201, 209–218. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knapton O (2016). Dynamic conceptualizations of threat in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Language and Cognition, 8, 1–31. 10.1017/langcog.2015.18 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kotov R, Perlman G, Gámez W, & Watson D (2015). The structure and short-term stability of the emotional disorders: A dimensional approach. Psychological Medicine, 45, 1687–1698. 10.1017/S0033291714002815 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, & Watson D (2010). Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 768–821. 10.1037/a0020327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotov R, Krueger RF, Watson D, Achenbach TM, Althoff RR, Bagby RM, Brown TA, Carpenter WT, Caspi A, Clark LA, Eaton NR, Forbes MK, Forbush KT, Goldberg D, Hasin D, Hyman SE, Ivanova MY, Lynam DR, Markon K, … Zimmerman M (2017). The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 454–477. 10.1037/abn0000258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotov R, Ruggero CJ, Krueger RF, Watson D, Yuan Q, & Zimmerman M (2011). New dimensions in the quantitative classification of mental illness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 1003–1011. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotov R, Watson D, Robles JP, & Schmidt NB (2007). Personality traits and anxiety symptoms: The multilevel trait predictor model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1485–1503. 10.1016/j.brat.2006.11.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kozak MJ, Foa EB, & Steketee G (1988). Process and outcome of exposure treatment with obsessive-compulsives: Psychophysiological indicators of emotional processing. Behavior Therapy, 19, 157–169. 10.1016/S0005-7894(88)80039-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Krabbe S, Gründemann J, & Lüthi A (2018). Amygdala inhibitory circuits regulate associative fear conditioning. Biological Psychiatry, 83, 800–809. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krompinger JW, Van Kirk NP, Garner LE, Potluri SI, & Elias JA (2019). Hope for the worst: Occasional reinforced extinction and expectancy violation in the treatment of OCD. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 26, 143–153. 10.1016/j.cbpra.2017.12.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RF, Kotov R, Watson D, Forbes MK, Eaton NR, Ruggero CJ, Simms LJ, Widiger TA, Achenbach TM, Bach B, Bagby RM, Bornovalova MA, Carpenter WT, Chmielewski M, Cicero DC, Clark LA, Conway C, DeClercq B, DeYoung CG, … Zimmermann J (2018). Progress in achieving quantitative classification of psychopathology. World Psychiatry, 17, 282–293. 10.1002/wps.20566 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RF, & Markon KE (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity: A model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 111–133. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange I, Goossens L, Bakker J, Michielse S, Marcelis M, Wichers M, van Os J, van Amelsvoort T, & Schruers K (2019). Functional neuroimaging of associative learning and generalization in specific phobia. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 89, 275–285. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leckman JF, Grice DE, Boardman J, Zhang H, Vitale A, Bondi C, … & Pauls DL (1997). Symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 911–917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux J, & Daw ND (2018). Surviving threats: Neural circuit and computational implications of a new taxonomy of defensive behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 19, 269–282. 10.1038/nrn.2018.22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux JE, Moscarello J, Sears R, & Campese V (2017). The birth, death and resurrection of avoidance: A reconceptualization of a troubled paradigm. Molecular Psychiatry, 22, 24–36. 10.1038/mp.2016.166 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leech R, & Sharp DJ (2014). The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain, 137, 12–32. 10.1093/brain/awt162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liew J, Grisham JR, & Hayes BK (2018). Inductive and deductive reasoning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 59, 79–86. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2017.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lissek S (2012). Toward an account of clinical anxiety predicated on basic, neurally mapped mechanisms of Pavlovian fear-learning: The case for conditioned overgeneralization. Depression and Anxiety, 29, 257–263. 10.1002/da.21922 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lissek S, Pine DS, & Grillon C (2006). The strong situation: A potential impediment to studying the psychobiology and pharmacology of anxiety disorders. Biological Psychology, 72, 265–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lissek S, Powers AS, McClure EB, Phelps EA, Woldehawariat G, Grillon C, & Pine DS (2005). Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1391–1424. 10.1016/j.brat.2004.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lissek S, & van Meurs B (2014). Learning models of PTSD: Theoretical accounts and psychobiological evidence. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.11.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lochner C, & Stein DJ (2003). Heterogeneity of obsessive-compulsive disorder: A literature review. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 11, 113–132. 10.1080/10673220303949 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lonsdorf TB, Menz MM, Andreatta M, Fullana MA, Golkar A, Haaker J, Heitland I, Hermann A, Kuhn M, Kruse O, Meir Drexler S, Meulders A, Nees F, Pittig A, Richter J, Römer S, Shiban Y, Schmitz A, Straube B, … Merz CJ (2017). Don’t fear ‘fear conditioning’: Methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of fear. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 77, 247–285. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lonsdorf TB, & Merz CJ (2017). More than just noise: Inter-individual differences in fear acquisition, extinction and return of fear in humans - Biological, experiential, temperamental factors, and methodological pitfalls. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 703–728. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lonsdorf TB, Merz CJ, & Fullana MA (2019). Fear extinction retention: Is it what we think it is? Biological Psychiatry, 85, 1074–1082. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackintosh NJ (1974). The psychology of animal learning. Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Maren S (2001). Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 897–931. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.897 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marin M-F, Zsido RG, Song H, Lasko NB, Killgore WDS, Rauch SL, Simon NM, & Milad MR (2017). Skin conductance responses and neural activations during fear conditioning and extinction recall across anxiety disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markarian Y, Larson MJ, Aldea MA, Baldwin SA, Good D, Berkeljon A, Murphy TK, Storch EA, & McKay D (2010). Multiple pathways to functional impairment in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 78–88. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marks IM, Crowe M, Drewe E, Young J, & Dewhurst WG (1969). Obsessive compulsive neurosis in identical twins. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 115(526), 991–998. 10.1192/bjp.115.526.991 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S (2014). Nature and causes of the immediate extinction deficit: A brief review. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 113, 19–24. 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mataix-Cols D, Fernández de la Cruz L, Monzani B, Rosenfield D, Andersson E, Pérez-Vigil A, Frumento P, de Kleine RA, Difede J, Dunlop BW, Farrell LJ, Geller D, Gerardi M, Guastella AJ, Hofmann SG, Hendriks G-J, Kushner MG, Lee FS, Lenze EJ, … Thuras P (2017). D-cycloserine augmentation of exposure-based cognitive behavior therapy for anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, and posttraumatic stress disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis of individual participant data. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 501. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3955 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.McGuire JF, Orr SP, Wu MS, Lewin AB, Small BJ, Phares V, Murphy TK, Wilhelm S, Pine DS, Geller D, & Storch EA (2016). Fear conditioning and extinction in youth with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 33, 229–237. 10.1002/da.22468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire JF, Storch EA, Lewin AB, Price LH, Rasmussen SA, & Goodman WK (2012). The role of avoidance in the phenomenology of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53, 187–194. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire JF, Wu MS, Piacentini J, McCracken JT, & Storch EA (2017). A Meta-analysis of D-cycloserine in exposure-based treatment: Moderators of treatment efficacy, response, and diagnostic remission. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 78, 196–206. 10.4088/JCP.15r10334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKay D, Abramowitz JS, Calamari JE, Kyrios M, Radomsky A, Sookman D, Taylor S, & Wilhelm S (2004). A critical evaluation of obsessive–compulsive disorder subtypes: Symptoms versus mechanisms. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 283–313. 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.McLaughlin NCR, Strong D, Abrantes A, Garnaat S, Cerny A, O’Connell C, Fadok R, Spofford C, Rasmussen SA, Milad MR, & Greenberg BD (2015). Extinction retention and fear renewal in a lifetime obsessive–compulsive disorder sample. Behavioural Brain Research, 280, 72–77. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.11.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menzies L, Chamberlain SR, Laird AR, Thelen SM, Sahakian BJ, & Bullmore ET (2008). Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: The orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 525–549. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer V (1966). Modification of expectations in cases with obsessional rituals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 4, 273–280. 10.1016/0005-7967(66)90023-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Milad MR, Furtak SC, Greenberg JL, Keshaviah A, Im JJ, Falkenstein MJ, Jenike M, Rauch SL, & Wilhelm S (2013). Deficits in conditioned fear extinction in obsessive-compulsive disorder and neurobiological changes in the fear circuit. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 608–618. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.914 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Orr SP, Lasko NB, Chang Y, Rauch SL, & Pitman RK (2008). Presence and acquired origin of reduced recall for fear extinction in PTSD: Results of a twin study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42, 515–520. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.01.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Pitman RK, Ellis CB, Gold AL, Shin LM, Lasko NB, Zeidan MA, Handwerger K, Orr SP, & Rauch SL (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 1075–1082. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, & Quirk GJ (2012). Fear extinction as a model for translational neuroscience: ten years of progress. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129–151. 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131631 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Quirk GJ, Pitman RK, Orr SP, Fischl B, & Rauch SL (2007). A role for the human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in fear expression. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 1191–1194. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, & Rauch SL (2012). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Beyond segregated cortico-striatal pathways. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 43–51. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mineka S, & Zinbarg R (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: It’s not what you thought it was. American Psychologist, 61, 10–26. 10.1037/0003-066X.61.1.10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morein-Zamir S, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, & Sahakian BJ (2010). Inhibition of thoughts and actions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Extending the endophenotype? Psychological Medicine, 40, 263–272. 10.1017/S003329170999033X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, The PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Med, 6, e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed1000097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morey RA, Haswell CC, Stjepanović D, Dunsmoor JE, LaBar KS, & Mid-Atlantic MIRECC Workgroup. (2020). Neural correlates of conceptual-level fear generalization in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 45, 1380–1389. 10.1038/s41386-020-0661-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mowrer OH (1939). A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Psychological Review, 46, 553–565. 10.1037/h0054288 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mowrer OH (1951). Two-factor learning theory: Summary and comment. Psychological Review, 58, 350–354. 10.1037/h0058956 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mowrer OH (1947). On the dual nature of learning—A re-interpretation of “conditioning” and “problem-solving.” Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102–148. [Google Scholar]
- Myers CE, & Gluck MA (1994). Context, conditioning, and hippocampal representation in animal learning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 835–847. 10.1037/0735-7044.108.5.835 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers KM, & Davis M (2004). AX+, BX- discrimination learning in the fear-potentiated startle paradigm: Possible relevance to inhibitory fear learning in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 464–475. 10.1101/lm.74704 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ng KH, & Freeman JH (2014). Amygdala inactivation impairs eyeblink conditioning in developing rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 999–1007. 10.1002/dev.21180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Nanbu M, Kurayama T, Nakazawa K, Matsuzawa D, Komiya Z, Haraguchi T, … & Shimizu E (2010). Impaired P50 suppression in fear extinction in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 34, 317–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nielen MMA, Veltman DJ, de Jong R, Mulder G, & den Boer JA (2002). Decision making performance in obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 69, 257–260. 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00381-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nielen MM, den Boer JA, & Smid HGOM (2009). Patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder are impaired in associative learning based on external feedback. Psychological Medicine, 39, 1519–1526. 10.1017/S0033291709005297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norberg MM, Krystal JH, & Tolin DF (2008). A meta-analysis of D-cycloserine and the facilitation of fear extinction and exposure therapy. Biological Psychiatry, 63(12), 1118–1126. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor K (2002). Intrusions and inferences in obsessive compulsive disorder. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 9, 38–46. 10.1002/cpp.303 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ojalehto HJ, Abramowitz JS, Hellberg SN, Buchholz JL, & Twohig MP (2020). Adherence to exposure and response prevention as a predictor of improvement in obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 72, 102210. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102210 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olatunji BO, Berg H, Cox RC, & Billingsley A (2017). The effects of cognitive reappraisal on conditioned disgust in contamination-based OCD: An analogue study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 51, 86–93. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2017.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olatunji BO, Forsyth JP, & Cherian A (2007). Evaluative differential conditioning of disgust: A sticky form of relational learning that is resistant to extinction. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 820–834. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olatunji BO, Lohr JM, Sawchuk CN, & Tolin DF (2007). Multimodal assessment of disgust in contamination-related obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 263–276. 10.1016/j.brat.2006.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olatunji BO, Lohr JM, Smits JAJ, Sawchuk CN, & Patten K (2009). Evaluative conditioning of fear and disgust in blood-injection-injury phobia: Specificity and impact of individual differences in disgust sensitivity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 153–159. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olatunji BO, Rosenfield D, Tart CD, Cottraux J, Powers MB, & Smits JAJ (2013). Behavioral versus cognitive treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder: An examination of outcome and mediators of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81, 415–428. 10.1037/a0031865 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr SP, Metzger LJ, Lasko NB, Macklin ML, Peri T, & Pitman RK (2000). De novo conditioning in trauma-exposed individuals with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 290–298. 10.1037/0021-843X.109.2.290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otto MW, Moshier SJ, Kinner DG, Simon NM, Pollack MH, & Orr SP (2014). De novo fear conditioning across diagnostic groups in the affective disorders: evidence for learning impairments. Behavior Therapy, 45, 619–629. 10.1016/j.beth.2013.12.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov IP (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Courier Dover Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Pélissier M-C, & O’Connor KP (2002). Deductive and inductive reasoning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 15–27. 10.1348/014466502163769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phelps EA, Delgado MR, Nearing KI, & LeDoux JE (2004). Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron, 43, 897–905. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinto A, Greenberg BD, Grados MA, Bienvenu OJ, Samuels JF, Murphy DL, Hasler G, Stout RL, Rauch SL, Shugart YY, Pauls DL, Knowles JA, Fyer AJ, McCracken JT, Piacentini J, Wang Y, Willour VL, Cullen B, Liang K-Y, … Nestadt G (2008). Further development of YBOCS dimensions in the OCD Collaborative Genetics Study: Symptoms vs. categories. Psychiatry Research, 160, 83–93. 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitman RK (1993). Posttraumatic obsessive-compulsive disorder: A case study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34, 102–107. 10.1016/0010-440X(93)90054-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitman RK, Rasmusson AM, Koenen KC, Shin LM, Orr SP, Gilbertson MW, … & Liberzon I (2012). Biological studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 769–787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pittig A, & Scherbaum S (2019). Costly avoidance in anxious individuals: Elevated threat avoidance in anxious individuals under high, but not low competing rewards. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 101524. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.101524 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pittig A, Schulz AR, Craske MG, & Alpers GW (2014). Acquisition of behavioral avoidance: Task-irrelevant conditioned stimuli trigger costly decisions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123, 314–329. 10.1037/a0036136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pittig A, Treanor M, LeBeau RT, & Craske MG (2018). The role of associative fear and avoidance learning in anxiety disorders: Gaps and directions for future research. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 88, 117–140. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.03.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabinak CA, Mori S, Lyons M, Milad MR, & Phan KL (2017). Acquisition of CS-US contingencies during Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction in social anxiety disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.09.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachman S (1976). The passing of the two-stage theory of fear and avoidance: Fresh possibilities. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14, 125–131. 10.1016/0005-7967(76)90066-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachman S (2015). The evolution of behaviour therapy and cognitive behaviour therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 64, 1–8. 10.1016/j.brat.2014.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachman S, Hodgson R, & Marks IM (1971). The treatment of chronic obsessive-compulsive neurosis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 9, 237–247. 10.1016/0005-7967(71)90009-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachman S, Hodgson R, & Marzillier J (1970). Treatment of an obsessional-compulsive disorder by modelling. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8, 385–392. 10.1016/0005-7967(70)90058-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachman SJ, & Hodgson RJ (1980). Obsessions and compulsions. Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Raines AM, Allan NP, Oglesby ME, Short NA, & Schmidt NB (2015). Examination of the relations between Obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions and fear and distress disorder symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.05.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rassin E, Cougle JR, & Muris P (2007). Content difference between normal and abnormal obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2800–2803. 10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rattel JA, Miedl SF, Blechert J, & Wilhelm FH (2017). Higher threat avoidance costs reduce avoidance behaviour which in turn promotes fear extinction in humans. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 96, 37–46. 10.1016/j.brat.2016.12.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Remijnse PL, Nielen MMA, van Balkom AJLM, Cath DC, van Oppen P, Uylings HBM, & Veltman DJ (2006). Reduced orbitofrontal-striatal activity on a reversal learning task in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 1225–1236. 10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renna ME, Quintero JM, Fresco DM, & Mennin DS (2017). Emotion Regulation Therapy: A mechanism-targeted treatment for disorders of distress. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA (1969). Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 77–94. 10.1037/h0027760 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA (1988). Pavlovian conditioning. It’s not what you think it is. American Psychologist, 151–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA, & Holland PC (1977). Associations in Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Learning and Motivation, 8, 429–447. 10.1016/0023-9690(77)90044-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rosario-Campos MC, Miguel EC, Quatrano S, Chacon P, Ferrao Y, Findley D, Katsovich L, Scahill L, King RA, Woody SR, Tolin D, Hollander E, Kano Y, & Leckman JF (2006). The Dimensional Yale–Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale (DY-BOCS): An instrument for assessing obsessive–compulsive symptom dimensions. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 495–504. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001798 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosas-Vidal LE, Lozada-Miranda V, Cantres-Rosario Y, Vega-Medina A, Melendez L, & Quirk GJ (2018). Alteration of BDNF in the medial prefrontal cortex and the ventral hippocampus impairs extinction of avoidance. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43, 2636–2644. 10.1038/s41386-018-0176-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenfield D, Smits JAJ, Hofmann SG, Mataix-Cols D, de la Cruz LF, Andersson E, Rück C, Monzani B, Pérez-Vigil A, Frumento P, Davis M, de Kleine RA, Difede J, Dunlop BW, Farrell LJ, Geller D, Gerardi M, Guastella AJ, Hendriks G-J, … Otto MW (2019). Changes in dosing and dose timing of D-cycloserine explain its apparent declining efficacy for augmenting exposure therapy for anxiety-related disorders: An individual participant-data meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 68, 102149. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2019.102149 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salkovskis PM (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 571–583. 10.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salkovskis PM (1999). Understanding and treating obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, Supplement 1, S29–S52. 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00049-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salters-Pedneault K, Tull MT, & Roemer L (2004). The role of avoidance of emotional material in the anxiety disorders. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 11, 95–114. 10.1016/j.appsy.2004.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Savage HS, Davey CG, Fullana MA, & Harrison BJ (2020). Threat and safety reversal learning in social anxiety disorder – an fMRI study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 76, 102321. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxena S, & Rauch SL (2000). Functional neuroimaging and the neuroanatomy of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23, 563–586. 10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70181-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schienle A, Schäfer A, Stark R, Walter B, & Vaitl D (2005). Neural responses of OCD patients towards disorder-relevant, generally disgust-inducing and fear-inducing pictures. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 69–77. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.12.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller D, Levy I, Niv Y, LeDoux JE, & Phelps EA (2008). From fear to safety and back: Reversal of fear in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 11517–11525. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2265-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzmeier H, Kleint NI, Wittchen HU, Ströhle A, Hamm AO, & Lueken U (2019). Characterizing the nature of emotional-associative learning deficits in panic disorder: An fMRI study on fear conditioning, extinction training and recall. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 306–318. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.11.1108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shvil E, Sullivan GM, Schafer S, Markowitz JC, Campeas M, Wager TD, Milad MR, & Neria Y (2014). Sex differences in extinction recall in posttraumatic stress disorder: A pilot fMRI study. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 113, 101–108. 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sijbrandij M, Engelhard IM, Lommen MJJ, Leer A, & Baas JMP (2013). Impaired fear inhibition learning predicts the persistence of symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 1991–1997. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simpson HB, Huppert JD, Petkova E, Foa EB, & Liebowitz MR (2006). Response versus remission in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67, 269–276. 10.4088/JCP.v67n0214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soomro GM, Altman DG, Rajagopal S, & Browne MO (2008). Selective serotonin re- uptake inhibitors (SSRIs) versus placebo for obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 1. 10.1002/14651858.CD001765.pub3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern ER, Welsh RC, Fitzgerald KD, Gehring WJ, Lister JJ, Himle JA, Abelson JL, & Taylor SF (2011). Hyperactive error responses and altered connectivity in ventromedial and frontoinsular cortices in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 583–591. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storch EA, Geffken GR, & Murphy TK (2007). Handbook of Child and Adolescent Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Suarez-Jimenez B, Albajes-Eizagirre A, Lazarov A, Zhu X, Harrison BJ, Radua J, Neria Y, & Fullana MA (2019). Neural signatures of conditioning, extinction learning, and extinction recall in posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Psychological Medicine, 1–10. 10.1017/S0033291719001387 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Summerfeldt LJ, Kloosterman PH, Antony MM, & Swinson RP (2014). Examining an obsessive-compulsive core dimensions model: Structural validity of harm avoidance and incompleteness. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 3, 83–94. 10.1016/j.jocrd.2014.01.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Summerfeldt LJ, Richter MA, Antony MM, & Swinson RP (1999). Symptom structure in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A confirmatory factor-analytic study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 297–311. 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00134-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szechtman H, Ahmari SE, Beninger RJ, Eilam D, Harvey BH, Edemann-Callesen H, & Winter C (2017). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Insights from animal models. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 76, 254–279. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor S, Abramowitz JS, & McKay D (2007). Cognitive-behavioral models of obsessive-compulsive disorder. In Antony MM, Purdon C, & Summerfeldt LJ (Eds.), Psychological treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder: Fundamentals and beyond. (pp. 9–29). American Psychological Association. 10.1037/11543-001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor S, Abramowitz JS, McKay D, Calamari JE, Sookman D, Kyrios M, Wilhelm S, & Carmin C (2006). Do dysfunctional beliefs play a role in all types of obsessive–compulsive disorder? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 85–97. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teasdale JD (1974). Learning models of obsessional-compulsive disorder. In Beech HR (Ed.), Obsessional states. (1975–07516-009). Methuen & Co. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson RF, & Steinmetz JE (2009). The role of the cerebellum in classical conditioning of discrete behavioral responses. Neuroscience, 162, 732–755. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorsen AL, Hagland P, Radua J, Mataix-Cols D, Kvale G, Hansen B, & van den Heuvel OA (2018). Emotional processing in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of 25 functional neuroimaging studies. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3, 563–571. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.01.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tolin DF (2019). Inhibitory learning for anxiety-related disorders. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 26, 225–236. 10.1016/j.cbpra.2018.07.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *.Tracy JA, Ghose SS, Stecher T, McFall RM, & Steinmetz JE (1999). Classical conditioning in a nonclinical obsessive-compulsive population. Psychological Science, 10, 9–13. 10.1111/1467-9280.00098 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Uddin LQ (2015). Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 55–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Velzen LS, Vriend C, de Wit SJ, & van den Heuvel OA (2014). Response inhibition and interference control in obsessive–compulsive spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00419 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vansteenwegen D, Francken G, Vervliet B, De Clercq A, & Eelen P (2006). Resistance to extinction in evaluative conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 71–79. 10.1037/0097-7403.32.1.71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vervliet B, Baeyens F, Van den Bergh O, & Hermans D (2013). Extinction, generalization, and return of fear: A critical review of renewal research in humans. Biological Psychology, 92, 51–58. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vervliet B, & Boddez Y (2020). Memories of 100 years of human fear conditioning research and expectations for its future. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 103732. 10.1016/j.brat.2020.103732 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vervliet B, Craske MG, & Hermans D (2013). Fear extinction and relapse: State of the art. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 215–248. 10.1146/annurevclinpsy-050212-185542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vervoort E, Vervliet B, Bennett M, & Baeyens F (2014). Generalization of human fear acquisition and extinction within a novel arbitrary stimulus category. PLoS ONE, 9, e96569. 10.1371/journal.pone.0096569 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon V, Baek K, Enander J, Worbe Y, Morris LS, Harrison NA, Robbins TW, Rück C, & Daw N (2015). Motivation and value influences in the relative balance of goal-directed and habitual behaviours in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Translational Psychiatry, 5, e670–e670. 10.1038/tp.2015.165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Zhu Z, Hu J, Schiller D, & Li J (2021). Active suppression prevents the return of threat memory in humans. Communications Biology, 4, 1–8. 10.1038/s42003-021-02120-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson D (2005). Rethinking the mood and anxiety disorders: A quantitative hierarchical model for DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 522–536. 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheaton MG, Gershkovich M, Gallagher T, Foa EB, & Simpson HB (2018). Behavioral avoidance predicts treatment outcome with exposure and response prevention for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 35, 256–263. 10.1002/da.22720 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wisco BE, Sloan DM, & Marx BP (2013). Cognitive emotion regulation and written exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 435–442. 10.1177/2167702613486630 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xia J, Du Y, Han J, Liu G, & Wang X (2015). D-cycloserine augmentation in behavioral therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Drug Design, Development and Therapy, 9, 2101–2117. 10.2147/DDDT.S68994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuj DV, Palmer MA, Lommen MJJ, & Felmingham KL (2016). The centrality of fear extinction in linking risk factors to PTSD: A narrative review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 15–35. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]