Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Jul 13;18(7):e0286221. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0286221

Generalization of contextual fear is sex-specifically affected by high salt intake

Jasmin N Beaver 1,2, Brady L Weber 1,2, Matthew T Ford 1, Anna E Anello 1,2, Kaden M Ruffin 1, Sarah K Kassis 1,2, T Lee Gilman 1,2,3,*
Editor: Thiago P Fernandes4
PMCID: PMC10343085  PMID: 37440571

Abstract

A hallmark symptom of many anxiety disorders, and multiple neuropsychiatric disorders more broadly, is generalization of fearful responses to non-fearful stimuli. Anxiety disorders are often comorbid with cardiovascular diseases. One established, and modifiable, risk factor for cardiovascular diseases is salt intake. Yet, investigations into how excess salt consumption affects anxiety-relevant behaviors remains little explored. Moreover, no studies have yet assessed how high salt intake influences generalization of fear. Here, we used adult C57BL/6J mice of both sexes to evaluate the influence of two or six weeks of high salt consumption (4.0% NaCl), compared to controls (0.4% NaCl), on contextual fear acquisition, expression, and generalization. Further, we measured osmotic and physiological stress by quantifying serum osmolality and corticosterone levels, respectively. Consuming excess salt did not influence contextual fear acquisition nor discrimination between the context used for training and a novel, neutral context when training occurred 48 prior to testing. However, when a four week delay between training and testing was employed to induce natural fear generalization processes, we found that high salt intake selectively increases contextual fear generalization in females, but the same diet reduces contextual fear generalization in males. These sex-specific effects were independent of any changes in serum osmolality nor corticosterone levels, suggesting the behavioral shifts are a consequence of more subtle, neurophysiologic changes. This is the first evidence of salt consumption influencing contextual fear generalization, and adds information about sex-specific effects of salt that are largely missing from current literature.

Introduction

Excessive dietary salt intake occurs across nations and cultures [1, 2]. While salt’s practical uses include food preservation and flavor enhancement, consumption of too much salt has been associated with increased risk for cardiovascular diseases [36]. Indeed, average estimates of global salt intake indicate people consume 3,950 mg of sodium (the component of NaCl that has been associated with multiple negative health outcomes) [3, 7, 8]. When put in the context that, according to the World Health Organization, adults should consume less than 2,000 mg of sodium, this overconsumption of salt becomes quantifiably apparent [9]. It is estimated that reducing consumption of salt can attenuate risk for cardiovascular disease by ~20% [5, 10], meaning dietary salt consumption has a profound physiological influence.

Globally, cardiovascular diseases and neuropsychiatric disorders, particularly disorders involving anxiety symptoms, are often comorbid [1115] (see also reviews [16, 17]). Increasing literature suggests dietary components such as excess sugar and/or fat are associated with both metabolic conditions like insulin resistance and type II diabetes, as well as with neuropsychiatric conditions (e.g., depression) and diseases (e.g., Alzheimer’s) [1822]. Trait anxiety appears largely unaffected by excess salt consumption [2327] (reviewed in [28]). Far less understood is how excess salt intake might affect neuropsychiatric symptoms like generalized (i.e., non-specific, state) anxiety, though some researchers have begun to recognize this possibility [29, 30]. We hypothesized that excessive consumption of dietary salt would enhance fear generalization in rodents, a possibility that has not yet been explored.

Fear generalization is an established neurobehavioral phenomenon that occurs across species, and studies are making considerable progress in identifying brain regions critical to this process [3134]. These include specific subregions of the cingulate and prefrontal cortices, hippocampus, amygdala, and brainstem nuclei (e.g., locus coeruleus). Growing evidence indicates the hippocampus and amygdala are affected by excess salt consumption in rodents [24, 26, 3541], including: reductions in nicotinic receptor levels [35]; increased markers of oxidative stress [24, 26, 37, 41], neuronal activation [42], and neuroinflammation [42]; reduced long-term potentiation [40]; and increased microvasculature leakiness [36]. Other brain regions remain underexplored. This evidence, combined with human evidence of comorbidity between anxiety disorders and cardiovascular diseases, supports our hypothesis–at least based upon male data–that fear generalization is augmented by high salt intake. Unfortunately, both the scant literature on behavioral effects of increased salt intake, and the larger body of work on the neurocircuitry of fear generalization, have focused almost exclusively on male rodents. Here, we included both sexes in our studies, but given the current state of the literature we could not formulate data-based a priori hypotheses specifically regarding how female fear generalization behavior might differ from male fear generalization under conditions of excess salt intake.

Materials and methods

Animals

Mice of both sexes on a C57BL/6J background were purchased from Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME) and bred in-house. All mice were at least 9 weeks old prior to commencing experiments. At the start of diet manipulation, mice were singly housed to facilitate accurate quantification of diet and water consumption relative to the mouse’s body weight. Prior to experiments, all mouse cages contained Nestlets (Ancare, Bellmore, NY) and huts; at the time of singly housing for diet manipulation, two additional pieces of enrichment (e.g., swing, wood block, etc.) were added to each cage. Cages contained 7090 Teklad Sani-chip bedding (Envigo, East Millstone, NJ) and were housed in a 12:12 light/dark cycle room, with lights on at 07:00, and temperature maintained at 22 ± 2°C.

Prior to diet manipulation, mice were fed LabDiet 5001 rodent laboratory chow (LabDiet, Brentwood, MO) ad libitum. At the time of diet manipulation, mice were pseudorandomly assigned to either a control diet (0.4% NaCl w/w; D17012, Research Diets, Inc., New Brunswick, NJ) or a high salt diet (4.0% NaCl; D17013; Fig 1A). These diets were available ad libitum, and water was always available ad libitum. Twice weekly, measurements of diet and water consumption plus body weight were taken. Refer to Supporting Information for statistics and graphs on average water consumed per day (S15 and S19 Tables; S9 and S13 Figs); average food consumed per day (S16 and S20 Tables; S10 and S13 Figs); weekly body weight changes (S17 and S21 Tables; S11 and S14 Figs); raw body weights (S18 and S22 Tables; S12 and S14 Figs); average NaCl consumed per day (S23 and S28 Tables; S15 and S20 Figs); average NaCl consumed as percentage of body weight (S24 and S29 Tables; S16 and S20 Figs); weekly ratio between water:NaCl consumption (S25 and S30 Tables; S17 and S21 Figs); average kcal consumed per day (S26 and S31 Tables; S18 and S22 Figs); and average kcal consumed as percentage of body weight (S27 and S32 Tables; S19 and S22 Figs).

Fig 1. Study timeline and fear conditioning procedures.

Fig 1

A) Mice were assigned to either a control (0.4% NaCl) or high salt (4.0% NaCl) diet for two or six weeks. Experiment 1 involved diet manipulation for two weeks, at which time mice underwent context fear training, followed 48 h later by context fear testing. Experiment 2 (grey shading) involved diet manipulation for six weeks, at which time mice underwent context fear training, followed 48 h later by context fear testing. Experiment 3 involved diet manipulation for two weeks, at which time mice underwent context fear training, followed four weeks later (while continuing the same diet manipulation) by context fear testing. B) Mice trained with a mild foot shock in the Training Context were tested 48 h (Experiments 1, 2) or four weeks (Experiment 3) later in either the Training Context or the Neutral Context. The Training Context included a metal grid floor, visible illumination, patterned background, and 70% ethanol scent. The Neutral Context included a smooth acrylic floor, infrared illumination, no background, and Windex scent. C) Control no shock mice were exposed to the Training Context, but were never administered a foot shock. These mice were tested at the same timelines for Experiments 1–3 in the Training Context, to assess for any baseline influences of diet consumption on fear behavior.

Diet manipulations lasted for two weeks (Experiment 1; Fig 1A) or six weeks (Experiments 2 and 3; Fig 1A). All procedures were approved by the Kent State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol 536 LG 22–14), and adhered to the National Research Council’s Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, 8th Ed. [43]. Every measure was taken to minimize suffering.

Experimental timelines

Experimental timelines are graphically illustrated in Fig 1A. For Experiment 1, context fear training occurred after two weeks of diet manipulation, and context fear testing occurred 48 h after training. For Experiment 2, training occurred after six weeks of diet manipulation, and testing occurred 48 h after training. For Experiment 3, training occurred after two weeks of diet manipulation, and testing occurred four weeks after training (a standard time frame for natural development of fear generalization in mice [44]).

Context fear conditioning

Females and males always underwent behavior separately. Fear conditioning took place in identical Coulbourn Instruments chambers (7 in D × 7 in W × 12 in H; Allentown, PA) composed of two opposing aluminum walls and two Plexiglas walls surrounding a stainless-steel shock grid floor. Each chamber was located within its own sound-attenuating enclosure, and had a camera mounted to the top for real-time measurements of freezing behavior (absence of all movement except that necessary for breathing) using FreezeFrame (v. 5.201, Coulburn Instruments).

Context fear training

The Training Context (Fig 1B) consisted of the shock grid floor, a patterned background, visible illumination, and scent cue (70% ethanol). Training occurred at either the two week or six week time point (Fig 1A). Training involved exposure to five, 1 s, 0.8 mA scrambled foot shocks pseudorandomly administered during a 6 min training session at 137, 186, 229, 285, and 324 s. Training with five foot shocks is optimal for eliciting time-dependent contextual fear generalization in rodents [4446]. Freezing behavior was quantified as the average percent freezing across the first six 5 s bins (i.e., 30 s) that occurred immediately after the 5 s bin containing the foot shock.

Context fear testing

Testing occurred at either the two (Experiment 1) or six week (Experiments 2 and 3) time point (Fig 1A). Mice were tested either in the Training Context, or in a Neutral Context which consisted of a smooth floor, no background, only infrared illumination, and a different scent cue (Windex®, SC Johnson, Racine, WI; Fig 1B). All mice were only ever tested in a single context, and testing lasted for 10 minutes, with the average percentage freezing occurring during minutes two through six being quantified and analyzed [46]. Foot shocks were never administered during testing.

No shock controls

Control ‘no shock’ mice were run concurrently in every cohort of animals (Fig 1C) for every Experiment. These no shock mice were exposed to the Training Context for ‘training’, but no foot shock was ever administered. Likewise, control no shock mice were tested in the Training Context, to evaluate how diet manipulation and the Experiment timelines influenced freezing behavior in the absence of any contextual fear conditioning.

Serum measurements

Forty-eight hours after testing for context fear expression, mice were briefly anesthetized with isoflurane then rapidly decapitated for collection of trunk blood. Trunk blood was allowed to clot at room temperature for 30 min, then was spun at 3500 rpm for 1 h at 4°C. Serum was then removed from tubes and aliquoted into two vials–one for measuring serum osmolality, the other for measuring serum corticosterone. Serum osmolality was measured immediately using an Osmo1 single-sample micro-osmometer (Advanced Instruments, Norwood, MA). Prior to every serum batch, a ClinitrolTM 290 reference solution (Advanced Instruments) from a new ampule was measured to confirm accurate readings. Samples for serum corticosterone were stored immediately at −80°C until analysis with a corticosterone ELISA kit (Enzo Life Sciences, Inc., Farmingdale, NY) using their small volume protocol. Serum corticosterone levels were log-transformed to account for positive skewness [27, 47, 48]. Log-transformed serum corticosterone levels are, for brevity, hereafter referred to as ’cort’ levels.

Statistical analyses

Data were analyzed with GraphPad Prism 9.4.0 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA) and IBM SPSS Statistics 28.0.0.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY). Our a priori significance threshold was set at p<0.05. Analyses were made within each Experiment. Repeated measures data (diet/water consumption, salt or kcal calculations, body weight, context fear training) were analyzed using 3-way repeated measures ANOVAs within individual sexes ([repeated measure, e.g., body weight] × diet × context) and pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction. Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were employed for within-subjects analyses. Measurements of contextual fear expression, serum osmolality, and serum corticosterone across contexts were analyzed with a 3-way ANOVA (diet × sex × context) and Holm-Šídák’s post-hocs. Control no shock fear behavior data were analyzed separately, given these were purposely included as negative controls and not intended to be analyzed in comparison to experimental groups. These no shock control data were analyzed on their own with 3-way ANOVAs (diet × sex × context) and Holm-Šídák’s post-hocs, or with repeated measures 3-way ANOVAs within each sex ([repeated measure, e.g., body weight] × diet × sex) plus pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction, and also using Greenhouse-Geisser corrections for within-subjects analyses. All ANOVA statistics are reported in S1S32 Tables. The criterion to exclude outliers was a priori assigned as being >5 standard deviations ± mean. Numbers of mice per sex/shock/diet/context condition varied, and exact numbers are present in the respective figure legends for each group of data. Broadly: no shock fear behavior n = 7–9; shock fear behavior n = 7–10; shock serum osmolality n = 8–9; shock serum corticosterone n = 7–9. Details of outliers that were excluded (training baseline, n = 1; no shock testing, n = 1; shock testing, n = 4; osmolality, n = 1; log-transformed serum corticosterone, n = 2; water:NaCl consumption ratio across weeks, n = 8) are provided in S33 Table. Graphs were created with GraphPad Prism, and show the mean ± the 95% confidence interval (CI), with non-repeated measures graphs also showing individual data points.

Results

No shock controls were built into every Experiment to evaluate how consumption of a high salt (4.0% NaCl) diet, relative to the control (0.4% NaCl) diet, might affect fear behavior in the absence of contextual fear conditioning. For ‘training’ of no shock controls, no three-way interactions of time × sex × diet were observed in any of the three experiments, nor were two-way interactions of sex × diet nor time × diet (S1 Table). The only two-way interaction evaluated that reached the significance threshold was for a time × sex interaction in training for Experiment 3 (S1 Table), indicating that during ‘training’ one sex behaved differently over time than the other. In Experiments 1 and 2, no significant main effects of sex nor diet were detected, though both had significant main effects of time (S1 Table), meaning behavior of mice changed over the course of ‘training’. This is to be expected when mice are initially exploring a new environment, then become complacent in the absence of any apparent threat. Pairwise comparisons indicated a few time points at which significant differences in freezing behavior between males consuming control and high salt diets were detected (S1A Fig, left and right panels), but these did not exhibit a consistent pattern, and were absent in Experiment 2 (S1A Fig, middle panel), suggesting they were not meaningful.

As with ‘training’, testing of no shock control mice revealed no significant interactions involving diet, and no main effect of diet (S2 Table), illustrating that diet did not impact freezing behavior in the absence of any fear conditioning. A significant interaction between Experiment × sex was observed, but Holm-Šídák’s post-hoc tests revealed no significant differences across the sexes within any Experiment, nor within the sexes across any Experiments (S1B Fig), indicating at a granular level, there were no Experiment-specific sex differences.

Acquisition of contextual fear resulted in no significant three- nor two-way interactions in any of the three Experiments within either sex (S3S5 Tables). Likewise, no main effects of Context nor diet were detected (S3S5 Tables), meaning diet condition did not affect fear learning, and there were no inherent differences in mice to be tested in one Context or the other. In fact, the only significant main effect was that of time, detected across all three Experiments (S3S5 Tables). This illustrates all mice that underwent contextual fear conditioning with foot shocks present during training exhibited increasing levels of freezing (i.e., fear behavior) over the course of the training session (Fig 2). Pairwise comparisons between male mice to be tested in the Neutral Context indicated that, in Experiment 2, mice consuming high salt exhibited elevated freezing levels following the second and third of the five training foot shocks, relative to mice consuming control diet (Fig 2B, right panel). All other acquisition curves lacked significant differences across diets and future testing contexts (Fig 2), indicating that a high salt diet does not impact acquisition of contextual fear after two (Experiments 1, 3) or six (Experiment 2) weeks of consumption.

Fig 2. Context fear training across Experiments.

Fig 2

Mice assigned to 0.4% NaCl represented by blue symbols, mice assigned to 4.0% NaCl represented by red symbols; mice to be tested in Training Context represented by squares and solid lines, mice to be tested in Neutral Context represented by circles and dotted lines. All mice were trained in the Training Context with five, pseudorandomized mild (1 s, 0.8 mA) foot shocks during a 6 min training session. Percent freezing during training of mice in A) Experiment 1, B) Experiment 2 (grey shading), and C) Experiment 3. Baseline percent freezing averaged across the first two minutes of the training session and plotted along x-axis as 0 time point. Average percent freezing for each 30 sec period following each of the five mild foot shocks are plotted along x-axis as Post-shock Periods 1–5. Experiment 1: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 7; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 7; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8. Experiment 2: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 10. Experiment 3: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 7; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8. Data are graphed as mean ± 95% confidence interval. indicates p<0.05 difference between males to be tested in Neutral Context and consuming 0.4% and 4.0% NaCl diets at indicated time points.

Unlike context fear training, where diet was without significant effect on acquisition, high salt diet consumption did significantly affect context fear expression. Moreover, these dietary effects were often sex-selective. Across all three Experiments, there were no significant three-way interactions, nor were there any significant diet × context interactions (S7 Table). However, sex-specific fear behaviors between contexts were significant for Experiments 2 and 3, and fear behaviors across diet and sex combinations were significant for Experiment 3 (S7 Table). Further, diet alone had a significant effect in Experiment 2, and sex and context independently had significant effects on fear behavior in Experiment 1 (S7 Table). In other words, diet was without a significant effect in Experiment 1, but diet did influence the outcomes of Experiments 2 and 3, and for all three Experiments, sex had a significant–and mostly interactional–impact as well.

Holm-Šídák’s post-hoc testing helped indicate directionality of high salt consumption’s effects on contextual fear expression and generalization across sexes for the three different Experiment timelines employed. Experiment 1 involved training mice after two weeks of diet manipulation, and testing 48 h after training. The expected low (average ~20% or less) freezing levels in the Neutral Context, with higher (average ~40% or more) freezing levels in the Training Context, were observed across nearly all the sex and diet combinations (Fig 3A). A non-significant trend was noted for contextual fear expression in the Training Context between females and males consuming the high salt diet (p = 0.0542; Fig 3A). Excepting this, Experiment 1 suggests that contextual fear expression after two weeks of high salt diet consumption is unaffected across contexts and sexes.

Fig 3. Context fear expression across contexts and Experiments.

Fig 3

Mice assigned to 0.4% NaCl represented by blue symbols, mice assigned to 4.0% NaCl represented by red symbols; mice tested in Training Context represented by squares and solid lines, mice tested in Neutral Context represented by circles and dotted lines. Percent freezing during minutes two through six of the 10 min testing session are graphed for all mice. Testing occurred A) 48 h after training in Experiment 1, during which mice underwent two weeks of diet manipulation; B) 48 h after training in Experiment 2 (grey shading), during which mice underwent six weeks of diet manipulation; and C) four weeks after training in Experiment 3, during which mice underwent six total weeks of diet manipulation (training occurred after two weeks of diet manipulation). Experiment 1: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 7; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 7; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8. Experiment 2: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 10. Experiment 3: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 7; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8. Data are graphed as mean ± 95% confidence interval. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 indicate difference between Training Context and Testing Context within same sex and diet. ✣✣✣indicates p<0.001 difference between females and males on same diet and tested in same context.

Experiment 2 involved training mice after six weeks of diet manipulation, then testing 48 h following training. Similar to Experiment 1, contextual fear expression was largely unaffected by diet or sex, with one exception. Male mice consuming the control diet exhibited reduced contextual fear expression in the Training Context as compared to female mice consuming the control diet (Fig 3B); this sex difference in Training Context fear expression was absent in mice of both sexes consuming the high salt diet. As anticipated when testing for contextual fear expression relatively soon after training (in this case, 48 h), freezing behavior averaging ≤ 20% indicated that mice did not exhibit fear behavior in the Neutral Context (see S1 Fig for no shock comparisons).

Experiment 3 evaluated how high salt intake affects contextual fear generalization by training mice after two weeks of diet manipulation, then testing for contextual fear expression four weeks later (i.e., after 6 wks of diet manipulation). When doing this, contextual fear generalization (freezing ~40% or more in Neutral Context) was observed in all but males consuming a high salt diet (Fig 3C). In fact, contextual fear expression in females consuming a high salt diet tested in the Neutral Context was so high that it was statistically indifferent from females consuming either diet and tested in the Training Context (Fig 3C). On the contrary, males tested in the Neutral Context after consuming high salt exhibited significantly lower contextual fear than females tested in the Neutral Context after consuming high salt (Fig 3C). This indicates a bidirectional, sex-selective effect of high salt consumption on contextual fear generalization.

Regardless of diet condition, all mice had ad libitum access to drinking water. Nonetheless, we evaluated if serum osmolality was affected across diet condition, sex, or testing Context in mice trained with foot shocks (Fig 4A–4C), as well as across diet, sex, and Experiment in control no shock mice. In control no shock mice, no three-way interactions were observed, but a significant two-way interaction where serum osmolality across the sexes differed between Experiments (S11 Table). Despite this interaction, Holm-Šídák’s post-hoc testing revealed no significant directional differences when the data were examined between sexes within each Experiment for control no shock mice. Analyses of mice trained with foot shocks revealed zero three- or two-way interactions, and no significant main effects (S12 Table). Serum osmolality was not affected by diet, neither in control no shock mice nor in fear conditioned mice. Taken together, these findings suggest serum osmolality was unaltered in all mice, and thus the behavioral changes observed were not the consequence of osmotic stress. For food, water, salt, and kcal consumption data, plus body weight records, readers are referred to the Supporting Information.

Fig 4. Serum osmolality in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

Fig 4

Mice assigned to 0.4% NaCl represented by blue symbols, mice assigned to 4.0% NaCl represented by red symbols; mice tested in Training Context represented by squares and solid lines, mice tested in Neutral Context represented by circles and dotted lines. Serum osmolality in context fear conditioned mice from A) Experiment 1, B) Experiment 2 (grey shading), and C) Experiment 3. Experiment 1: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8. Experiment 2: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 9. Experiment 3: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 7; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 7; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 7. Data are graphed as mean ± 95% confidence interval.

In addition to serum osmolality, serum corticosterone levels were also measured in all mice 48 h after completion of context fear testing. Similar to serum osmolality, no three-way interactions were detected in log-transformed serum corticosterone (cort) levels of no shock control mice, but we did find a significant interaction between sexes across Experiments (S13 Table). Unlike with serum osmolality, Holm-Šídák’s post-hoc tests revealed directional, diet-specific differences in cort levels within sexes across Experiments, and across sexes within an Experiment (S6 Fig). Specifically, control-diet consuming females had lower cort levels in Experiment 2 relative to Experiment 1 (p = 0.0423). Similarly, high salt-consuming males had lower cort levels in Experiment 3 relative to Experiment 1 (p = 0.0109). Within Experiment 3, females consuming high salt had significantly higher cort levels relative to high salt males (p = 0.0143). Considering these changes were observed in mice that were not exposed to a foot shock at any point, these data suggest that six weeks of high salt consumption could sex-selectively reduce cort levels in males, but not females. However, the absence of a significant difference (p = 0.2369) between cort levels of high salt males between Experiments 1 and 2 challenges this interpretation.

Cort levels in mice that underwent context fear conditioning with foot shocks had no significant three- nor two-way significant interactions (S14 Table). Sex alone had a significant effect on cort levels in Experiments 1 and 3, whereas context specifically influenced cort levels in Experiment 2. Drilling down to directional differences, there were largely no significant differences detected by Holm-Šídák’s post-hoc tests (Fig 5A and 5B). The one exception was that in Experiment 3, males had lower cort levels than females (p = 0.0183), specifically between those consuming control diet and tested in the Training Context (Fig 5C). Similar, but non-significant, trends were noted between females and males tested in the Neutral Context consuming control diet (p = 0.0549) or consuming high salt (p = 0.0859; Fig 5C) for Experiment 3. Generally, cort levels did not consistently vary as a result of sex, diet, testing context, nor their interaction, indicating that shifts in circulating corticosterone are not responsible for the observed changes in contextual fear expression nor contextual fear generalization. Circulating cort levels were largely not correlated with contextual fear expression, except in females consuming high salt and tested in the Training Context in Experiment 3 (p = 0.039; see S23 Fig).

Fig 5. Log-transformed serum corticosterone levels in fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

Fig 5

Mice assigned to 0.4% NaCl represented by blue symbols, mice assigned to 4.0% NaCl represented by red symbols; mice tested in Training Context represented by squares and solid lines, mice tested in Neutral Context represented by circles and dotted lines. Log-transformed serum corticosterone levels in context fear conditioned mice in A) Experiment 1, B) Experiment 2 (grey shading), and C) Experiment 3. Experiment 1: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 6; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 7; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 7; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 7; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8. Experiment 2: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 7; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 7; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 9; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 9; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 10. Experiment 3: 0.4% NaCl females Training Context, n = 7; 0.4% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl females Neutral Context, n = 7; 0.4% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 0.4% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Training Context, n = 8; 4.0% NaCl males Neutral Context, n = 7. Data are graphed as mean ± 95% confidence interval. indicates p<0.05 difference between females and males on same diet and tested in same context.

Discussion

This is the first investigation into how context fear training, and subsequent expression and generalization, are affected across the sexes by a high salt diet. Our findings suggest that a relatively short (two week) period of high salt consumption might reduce expression of recently learned context fear in males, whereas a longer (six week) consumption period of high salt prevents decreases in expression of recently learned context fear in males. Moreover, context fear generalization was enhanced in females, but attenuated in males, by consumption of a high salt diet. Despite being contrary to our hypothesis regarding males, these data indicate that excess salt consumption affects behavior in a sex-selective manner. Further, these behavioral changes occurred independent of any changes in serum osmolality, and likewise largely did not map onto serum corticosterone levels, indicating that neither osmotic nor physiologic stress states were responsible.

Previous studies assessing how excess salt intake affects context fear behavior exclusively used male rats [25] or male mice [26]. These studies both used higher salt diets of 8.0% NaCl, compared to the 4.0% NaCl diet used here [25, 26]. Both teams reported attenuated context fear expression, indexed by freezing behavior. This freezing behavior was measured during testing that occurred in the Training Context either 24 h [26], or both 2 and 24 h [25], after training. Their observations differ from ours, which did not observe significant effects of high salt diet on contextual fear expression in the Training Context. A similarity between the present study and that of Ge and colleagues is the absence of any diet influence on fear acquisition to the auditory cue [26], or in our case, to the context. However, this raises an important caveat regarding both prior investigations—rodents were trained using a cued fear paradigm with an auditory tone [25, 26], rather than training the rodents as the present study did with a traditional context fear conditioning protocol that lacks discrete cues. These different training procedures engage distinct brain regions [49, 50], and likewise elicit distinguishable patterns of neuronal activation [5153]. Thus, in the present study we assessed contextual fear expression specifically following contextual fear conditioning, rather than cued fear conditioning.

Generalization of contextual fear is a hallmark symptom of many anxiety disorders, and is a conserved cross-species neurobehavioral phenomenon that is useful for understanding how neurophysiological changes influence behaviors [32, 54]. The majority of contextual fear generalization investigations in rodents have been performed in males [33], though studies have recently begun to include females. Such sex-inclusive studies indicate that rodents of both sexes can and do exhibit generalized contextual fear, at timepoints both days [55] and weeks [56] after context fear training. Sex differences in the influence of various contextual modalities have been reported (see review [33]), and in our study contextual differences included tactile, olfactory, and visual modalities. We observed robust discrimination between contexts across the sexes when training was temporally proximal to testing, as well as convincing evidence of contextual fear generalization in both sexes consuming control diet when testing occurred four weeks following training. In contrast to our hypothesis that high salt intake would enhance contextual fear generalization in males, we instead found an attenuation of this measure in males, but an enhancement in females. Given the near absence of investigations into how excess salt intake affects female behaviors (see review [28]), plus the relatively scant literature on contextual fear generalization in females (see review [33]), we did not have enough information to formulate an a priori hypothesis for females consuming high salt. Some researchers have suggested excess salt intake exacerbates neuropsychiatric symptoms and conditions [29, 30]. Our data support this relationship, at least in females. However, the relationship between high salt consumption and anxiety-relevant behaviors was not supported in our male mice, and in fact our data suggest an inverse relationship. Given frequent comorbidity between cardiovascular diseases and pathological anxiety [1115] (see also reviews [16, 17]), and the adverse impact of overconsumption of salt on cardiovascular disease risk [5, 10], this finding in males was surprising. Nonetheless, it may be that in males, high salt-mediated cardiovascular pathology is necessary for emergence of increased anxiety-related behaviors, whereas in females excess salt is sufficient. This would parallel findings indicating greater metabolic disruptions in men relative to women with comorbid obesity and mental health disorders [5759] (see review [60]), greater incidence of depression and anxiety in men versus women with multiple sclerosis [61], and increased incidence of a biomarker of cardiovascular disease in men versus women with depression [62]. However, some literature suggests this is not a consistent finding across disease conditions [63].

Along these lines, we evaluated if mice consuming excess salt exhibited peripheral indicators of osmotic (serum osmolality) or physiological (serum corticosterone) stress. Changes in these measures could suggest an overall disrupted state of homeostasis that might confound contextual fear learning and/or expression. For example, osmotic stress can reduce sensitivity to pain [64]. Though the shock level (0.8 mA) and duration (1 sec) that we use are intended to only elicit discomfort/distress, it remains possible that osmotic stress could skew sensory perception of this brief aversive experience. Similarly, endogenous corticosterone levels can influence temporal development of contextual fear generalization as well as the persistence of contextual discrimination [65]. The absence of any sex-, diet-, or context-specific differences in serum osmolality indicates that osmotic stress was not present in our experimental animals. No significant correlations between cort levels and fear expression in the Neutral Context were detected for either sex in Experiment 3. Indeed, of all the mice trained in the Training Context with foot shocks, only Experiment 3 females consuming high salt and tested in the Training Context displayed a significant (negative) correlation between context fear expression and cort levels (S23 Fig). The absence of any significant correlations between cort levels and any other sex/diet/context combination across Experiments indicates that cort levels overall probably did not confound observed context fear expression and generalization.

Limitations

Limitations of this study include singly housing of mice to allow for tracking of individual food/water/salt/kcal consumption and changes in body weight, meaning that mice were excluded from social stimulation. Some data suggests behavioral responses to single housing differs across the sexes in mice and rats (see review [66]), so this might have influenced sex-specific behaviors observed in the present study. However, a meta-analysis of 293 male and female mouse studies indicates that singly housing mice reduces coefficients of variation in trait variability by 37% in both sexes [67], suggesting our housing condition may have reduced the variability of our data. Diet manipulations were relatively short (two or six weeks), restricted to non-breeding adults, and contained no other accompanying unhealthy dietary components (e.g., low fiber, high fat). Only a single shock level and context fear conditioning paradigm was used, and mice were only tested in a single context. Thus, for future studies it will be important to evaluate behavior in group housed mice, assess longer diet manipulation periods and include additional unhealthy dietary components, and study how consumption of excess salt by younger mice affects their fear behaviors. Testing of behavior during the natural activity period of mice would be informative, as would assessing generalization and concurrent cort levels during different stages of the circadian cycle [68].

Conclusions

Our study provides foundational evidence of sex-selective consequences of excess salt intake on context fear expression and generalization. Additional studies will be essential to flesh out these initial findings and determine what brain cell populations are most impacted by excess salt intake, and how the effects of high salt might be mitigated. One logical starting point is evaluation of microglia, given converging evidence that both fear processing [69, 70] and high salt intake [42, 71, 72] affect microglial activation. Moreover, oral administration of the brain-penetrant anti-inflammatory drug minocycline can mitigate microglial activation and behavioral changes in rodents consuming high salt [42], and the same drug attenuated fear measures in human participants [73]. Future studies should also assess how excess salt intake affects cued fear expression and discrimination, and in so doing begin identifying how specific limbic brain regions might differentially be affected in their functionality and/or connectivity following extended periods of excess salt consumption. The evidence presented here, in conjunction with past literature focused on salt’s effects on behaviors outside of the fear domain(reviewed in [28]), indicates that salt is an underappreciated, non-caloric influence on conserved mammalian behaviors.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on context fear ‘training’ for control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S2 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on context fear testing of control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S3 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on context fear training for context fear conditioned mice of both sexes in Experiment 1.

(PDF)

S4 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on context fear training for context fear conditioned mice of both sexes in Experiment 2.

(PDF)

S5 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on context fear training for context fear conditioned mice of both sexes in Experiment 3.

(PDF)

S6 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on full 10 min time course of context fear testing for control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S7 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on context fear expression (min 2–6) for context fear conditioned mice across Experiment.

(PDF)

S8 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on full 10 min time course of context fear testing for mice of both sexes in Experiment 1.

(PDF)

S9 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on full 10 min time course of context fear testing for mice of both sexes in Experiment 2.

(PDF)

S10 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on full 10 min time course of context fear testing for mice of both sexes in Experiment 3.

(PDF)

S11 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on serum osmolality in control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S12 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on serum osmolality in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S13 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on log-transformed serum corticosterone levels in control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S14 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on log-transformed serum corticosterone in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S15 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average water consumption per day for context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S16 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average food consumption per day for context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S17 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly body weight change for context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S18 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on twice weekly body weight measurements of context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S19 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average water consumption per day for control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S20 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average food consumption per day for control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S21 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly body weight changes for control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S22 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on twice weekly body weights of control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S23 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average NaCl consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S24 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average NaCl consumed as a percentage of body weight in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S25 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly ratio of water to NaCl consumed in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S26 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average kcal consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S27 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly kcal consumed as a percentage of body weight in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S28 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average NaCl consumed per day by control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S29 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly NaCl consumed as a percentage of body weight by control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S30 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly water to NaCl ratio consumed by control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S31 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average kcal consumed per day by control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S32 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average kcal consumed as a percentage of body weight by control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S33 Table. Details of confirmed outliers (> 5 standard deviations (SD) ± average of data set sans suspected outlier) within respective data sets.

Outliers are marked with red font.

(PDF)

S1 Fig. Fear behavior in no shock control mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S2 Fig. Fear behavior in no shock control mice across Experiments with shortened Y axes.

(PDF)

S3 Fig. Time course of context fear expression testing in no shock groups.

(PDF)

S4 Fig. Time course of context fear expression testing in shock groups.

(PDF)

S5 Fig. Serum osmolality in no shock control mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S6 Fig. Log-transformed serum corticosterone levels of no shock control mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S7 Fig. Raw (pre-transformed) serum corticosterone levels of no shock control mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S8 Fig. Raw (pre-transformed) serum corticosterone levels in fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S9 Fig. Average water consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S10 Fig. Average food consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S11 Fig. Weekly body weight change in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S12 Fig. Body weight of context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S13 Fig. Average water and food consumed per day by control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S14 Fig. Body weights and body weight changes in control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S15 Fig. Average NaCl consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S16 Fig. NaCl consumed as a percentage of body weight by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S17 Fig. Water to NaCl consumed ratio for context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S18 Fig. Average kcal consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S19 Fig. Consumed kcal as a percentage of body weight by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S20 Fig. Average NaCl consumed per day and NaCl consumed as a percentage of body weight by control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S21 Fig. Ratio of water to NaCl consumed by control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S22 Fig. Average kcal consumed per day and kcal consumed as a percentage of body weight by control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

S23 Fig. Correlations between context fear expression and log-transformed serum corticosterone levels across contexts and Experiments.

(PDF)

S24 Fig. Correlations between context fear expression and log-transformed serum corticosterone levels in control no shock mice across Experiments.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the mice used in this study, and the expert care thereof by our caretakers. Figures were created with Biorender.com (Toronto ON) and GraphPad Prism (San Diego, CA).

Data Availability

The data are accessible at the following DOI: 10.17605/OSF.IO/E5K2M.

Funding Statement

Funding for this work was provided by Kent State University, and the Applied Psychology Center in the Department of Psychological Sciences at Kent State University.

References

  • 1.Powles J, Fahimi S, Micha R, Khatibzadeh S, Shi P, Ezzati M, et al. Global, regional and national sodium intakes in 1990 and 2010: a systematic analysis of 24 h urinary sodium excretion and dietary surveys worldwide. Bmj Open. 2013;3: e003733. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-003733 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Santos JA, Tekle D, Rosewarne E, Flexner N, Cobb L, Al-Jawaldeh A, et al. A Systematic Review of Salt Reduction Initiatives Around the World: A Midterm Evaluation of Progress Towards the 2025 Global Non-Communicable Diseases Salt Reduction Target. Adv Nutr. 2021;12: 1768–1780. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmab008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bibbins-Domingo K, Chertow GM, Coxson PG, Moran A, Lightwood JM, Pletcher MJ, et al. Projected Effect of Dietary Salt Reductions on Future Cardiovascular Disease. New Engl J Medicine. 2010;362: 590–599. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0907355 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cogswell ME, Mugavero K, Bowman BA, Frieden TR. Dietary Sodium and Cardiovascular Disease Risk—Measurement Matters. New Engl J Medicine. 2016;375: 580–586. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsb1607161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Adler AJ, Taylor F, Martin N, Gottlieb S, Taylor RS, Ebrahim S. Reduced dietary salt for the prevention of cardiovascular disease. Cochrane Db Syst Rev. 2014; CD009217. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009217.pub3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.He FJ, Tan M, Ma Y, MacGregor GA. Salt Reduction to Prevent Hypertension and Cardiovascular Disease. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2020;75: 632–647. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2019.11.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Mozaffarian D, Fahimi S, Singh GM, Micha R, Khatibzadeh S, Engell RE, et al. Global Sodium Consumption and Death from Cardiovascular Causes. New Engl J Medicine. 2014;371: 624–634. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1304127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.O’Donnell M, Mente A, Yusuf S. Sodium Intake and Cardiovascular Health. Circ Res. 2015;116: 1046–1057. doi: 10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.116.303771 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.World Health Organization. WHO global sodium benchmarks for different food categories. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2021. Available: https://apps.who.int/iris/rest/bitstreams/1344511/retrieve [Google Scholar]
  • 10.He FJ, MacGregor GA. Salt reduction lowers cardiovascular risk: meta-analysis of outcome trials. Lancet. 2011;378: 380–382. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61174-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Scherrer JF, Salas J, Cohen BE, Schnurr PP, Schneider FD, Chard KM, et al. Comorbid Conditions Explain the Association Between Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Incident Cardiovascular Disease. J Am Hear Assoc Cardiovasc Cerebrovasc Dis. 2019;8: e011133. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.118.011133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Szlejf C, Suemoto CK, Santos IS, Brunoni AR, Nunes MA, Viana MC, et al. Poorer cardiovascular health is associated with psychiatric comorbidity: results from the ELSA-Brasil Study. Int J Cardiol. 2019;274: 358–365. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2018.06.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tran J, Norton R, Conrad N, Rahimian F, Canoy D, Nazarzadeh M, et al. Patterns and temporal trends of comorbidity among adult patients with incident cardiovascular disease in the UK between 2000 and 2014: A population-based cohort study. Plos Med. 2018;15: e1002513. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002513 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Dhital PS, Sharma K, Poudel P, Dhital PR. Anxiety and Depression among Patients with Coronary Artery Disease Attending at a Cardiac Center, Kathmandu, Nepal. Nurs Res Pract. 2018;2018: 4181952. doi: 10.1155/2018/4181952 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vidal C, Polo R, Alvarez K, Falgas-Bague I, Wang Y, Cook BL, et al. Co-Occurrence of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Cardiovascular Disease Among Ethnic&sol;Racial Groups in the United States. Psychosom Med. 2018;80: 680–688. doi: 10.1097/psy.0000000000000601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Meuret AE, Tunnell N, Roque A. Anxiety Disorders, Rethinking and Understanding Recent Discoveries. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2020;1191: 237–261. doi: 10.1007/978-981-32-9705-0_15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Johnson HM. Anxiety and Hypertension: Is There a Link? A Literature Review of the Comorbidity Relationship Between Anxiety and Hypertension. Curr Hypertens Rep. 2019;21: 66. doi: 10.1007/s11906-019-0972-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Walker JM, Harrison FE. Shared Neuropathological Characteristics of Obesity, Type 2 Diabetes and Alzheimer’s Disease: Impacts on Cognitive Decline. Nutrients. 2015;7: 7332–7357. doi: 10.3390/nu7095341 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Tumminia A, Vinciguerra F, Parisi M, Frittitta L. Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus and Alzheimer’s Disease: Role of Insulin Signalling and Therapeutic Implications. Int J Mol Sci. 2018;19: 3306. doi: 10.3390/ijms19113306 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Subba R, Sandhir R, Singh SP, Mallick BN, Mondal AC. Pathophysiology linking depression and type 2 diabetes: Psychotherapy, physical exercise, and fecal microbiome transplantation as damage control. Eur J Neurosci. 2021;53: 2870–2900. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Joseph JJ, Golden SH. Cortisol dysregulation: the bidirectional link between stress, depression, and type 2 diabetes mellitus. Ann Ny Acad Sci. 2017;1391: 20–34. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13217 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Silva N de ML e, Lam MP, Soares CN, Munoz DP, Milev R, Felice FGD. Insulin Resistance as a Shared Pathogenic Mechanism Between Depression and Type 2 Diabetes. Frontiers Psychiatry. 2019;10: 57. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ernsberger P, Azar S, Iwai J. Open-field behavior in two models of genetic hypertension and the behavioral effects of salt excess. Behav Neural Biol. 1983;37: 46–60. doi: 10.1016/s0163-1047(83)91061-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Chugh G, Asghar M, Patki G, Bohat R, Jafri F, Allam F, et al. A High-Salt Diet Further Impairs Age-Associated Declines in Cognitive, Behavioral, and Cardiovascular Functions in Male Fischer Brown Norway Rats. J Nutrition. 2013;143: 1406–1413. doi: 10.3945/jn.113.177980 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Guo C, Wei Z, Huang F, Qin M, Li X, Wang Y, et al. High salt induced hypertension leads to cognitive defect. Oncotarget. 2017;5: 95780–95790. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.21326 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ge Q, Wang Z, Wu Y, Huo Q, Qian Z, Tian Z, et al. High salt diet impairs memory‐related synaptic plasticity via increased oxidative stress and suppressed synaptic protein expression. Mol Nutr Food Res. 2017;61: 1700134. doi: 10.1002/mnfr.201700134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Gilman TL, George CM, Andrade MA, Mitchell NC, Toney GM, Daws LC. High Salt Intake Lowers Behavioral Inhibition. Front Behav Neurosci. 2019;13: 271. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00271 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Beaver JN, Gilman TL. Salt as a non-caloric behavioral modifier: A review of evidence from pre-clinical studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2022;135: 104385. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Abdoli A. Hypothesis: High salt intake as an inflammation amplifier might be involved in the pathogenesis of neuropsychiatric disorders. Clin Exp Neuroimmunol. 2017;8: 146–157. doi: 10.1111/cen3.12389 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Jones AYM, Dean E, Lo SK. Interrelationships Between Anxiety, Lifestyle Self-reports and Fitness in a Sample of Hong Kong University Students. Ann Ny Acad Sci. 2002;5: 65–71. doi: 10.1080/102538902900012350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Webler RD, Berg H, Fhong K, Tuominen L, Holt DJ, Morey RA, et al. The Neurobiology of Human Fear Generalization: Meta-Analysis and Working Neural Model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;128: 421–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.06.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Dunsmoor JE, Paz R. Fear Generalization and Anxiety: Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms. Biol Psychiat. 2015;78: 336–343. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Asok A, Kandel ER, Rayman JB. The Neurobiology of Fear Generalization. Front Behav Neurosci. 2019;12: 329. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Jasnow AM, Lynch JF, Gilman TL, Riccio DC. Perspectives on fear generalization and its implications for emotional disorders. J Neurosci Res. 2017;95: 821–835. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23837 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Terry AV, Hernandez CM, Buccafusco JJ. Dahl salt-sensitive and salt-resistant rats: examination of learning and memory performance, blood pressure, and the expression of central nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. Neuroscience. 2001;103: 351–363. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00569-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Pelisch N, Hosomi N, Ueno M, Nakano D, Hitomi H, Mogi M, et al. Blockade of AT1 Receptors Protects the Blood–Brain Barrier and Improves Cognition in Dahl Salt-Sensitive Hypertensive Rats. Am J Hypertens. 2011;24: 362–368. doi: 10.1038/ajh.2010.241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Liu Y-Z, Chen J-K, Li Z-P, Zhao T, Ni M, Li D-J, et al. High-salt diet enhances hippocampal oxidative stress and cognitive impairment in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;114: 10–15. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Faraco G, Brea D, Garcia-Bonilla L, Wang G, Racchumi G, Chang H, et al. Dietary salt promotes neurovascular and cognitive dysfunction through a gut-initiated TH17 response. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21: 240–249. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0059-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mitchell NC, Gilman TL, Daws LC, Toney GM. High Salt Intake Enhances Swim Stress-Induced PVN Vasopressin Cell Activation and Active Stress Coping. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2018;93: 29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ge Q, Hu X, Ma N, Sun M, Zhang L, Cai Z, et al. Maternal high‐salt diet during pregnancy impairs synaptic plasticity and memory in offspring. Faseb J. 2021;35: e21244. doi: 10.1096/fj.202001890R [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Leal PE de PT, Silva AA da, Rocha-Gomes A, Riul TR, Cunha RA, Reichetzeder C, et al. High-Salt Diet in the Pre- and Postweaning Periods Leads to Amygdala Oxidative Stress and Changes in Locomotion and Anxiety-Like Behaviors of Male Wistar Rats. Front Behav Neurosci. 2022;15: 779080. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.779080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Gilman TL, Mitchell NC, Daws LC, Toney GM. Neuroinflammation contributes to high salt intake-augmented neuronal activation and active coping responses to acute stress. Int J Neuropsychoph. 2018;22: pyy099-. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyy099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.National Research Council. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. 8th ed.Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press (US); 2011. doi: 10.17226/12910 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cullen PK, Gilman TL, Winiecki P, Riccio DC, Jasnow AM. Activity of the anterior cingulate cortex and ventral hippocampus underlie increases in contextual fear generalization. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2015;124: 19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Poulos AM, Mehta N, Lu B, Amir D, Livingston B, Santarelli A, et al. Conditioning- and time-dependent increases in context fear and generalization. Learn Memory. 2016;23: 379–385. doi: 10.1101/lm.041400.115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lynch JF, Winiecki P, Gilman TL, Adkins JM, Jasnow AM. Hippocampal GABAB(1a) Receptors Constrain Generalized Contextual Fear. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;42: 914–924. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.255 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Uarquin DG, Meyer JS, Cardenas FP, Rojas MJ. Effect of Overcrowding on Hair Corticosterone Concentrations in Juvenile Male Wistar Rats. J Am Assoc Laboratory Animal Sci Jaalas. 2016;55: 749–755. Available: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27931312/ [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Teilmann AC, Kalliokoski O, Sørensen DB, Hau J, Abelson KSP. Manual versus automated blood sampling: impact of repeated blood sampling on stress parameters and behavior in male NMRI mice. Lab Anim. 2014;48: 278–291. doi: 10.1177/0023677214541438 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Differential Contribution of Amygdala and Hippocampus to Cued and Contextual Fear Conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106: 274–285. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.2.274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Curzon P, Rustay NR, Browman KE. Cued and Contextual Fear Conditioning for Rodents. CRC Press/Taylor & Francis, Boca Raton (FL); 2011. Available: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK5223 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Trogrlic L, Wilson YM, Newman AG, Murphy M. Context fear learning specifically activates distinct populations of neurons in amygdala and hypothalamus. Learn Memory. 2011;18: 678–687. doi: 10.1101/lm.2314311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Butler CW, Wilson YM, Gunnersen JM, Murphy M. Tracking the fear memory engram: discrete populations of neurons within amygdala, hypothalamus, and lateral septum are specifically activated by auditory fear conditioning. Learn Memory. 2015;22: 370–384. doi: 10.1101/lm.037663.114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Chaaya N, Jacques A, Belmer A, Richard DJ, Bartlett SE, Battle AR, et al. Localization of Contextual and Context Removed Auditory Fear Memory within the Basolateral Amygdala Complex. Neuroscience. 2019;398: 231–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Cooper SE, Dis EAM van, Hagenaars MA, Krypotos A-M, Nemeroff CB, Lissek S, et al. A meta-analysis of conditioned fear generalization in anxiety-related disorders. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2022;47: 1652–1661. doi: 10.1038/s41386-022-01332-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Keiser AA, Turnbull LM, Darian MA, Feldman DE, Song I, Tronson NC. Sex Differences in Context Fear Generalization and Recruitment of Hippocampus and Amygdala during Retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;42: 397–407. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.174 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Asok A, Hijazi J, Harvey LR, Kosmidis S, Kandel ER, Rayman JB. Sex Differences in Remote Contextual Fear Generalization in Mice. Front Behav Neurosci. 2019;13: 56. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Udo T, McKee SA, White MA, Masheb RM, Barnes RD, Grilo CM. Menopause and metabolic syndrome in obese individuals with binge eating disorder. Eat Behav. 2014;15: 182–185. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.01.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Bjerkeset O, Romundstad P, Evans J, Gunnell D. Association of Adult Body Mass Index and Height with Anxiety, Depression, and Suicide in the General PopulationThe HUNT Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167: 193–202. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm280 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Staiano AE, Marker AM, Martin CK, Katzmarzyk PT. Physical activity, mental health, and weight gain in a longitudinal observational cohort of nonobese young adults. Obesity. 2016;24: 1969–1975. doi: 10.1002/oby.21567 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Tronieri JS, Wurst CM, Pearl RL, Allison KC. Sex Differences in Obesity and Mental Health. Curr Psychiat Rep. 2017;19: 29. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0784-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Marrie RA, Patten SB, Tremlett H, Wolfson C, Warren S, Svenson LW, et al. Sex differences in comorbidity at diagnosis of multiple sclerosis A population-based study. Neurology. 2016;86: 1279–1286. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Garcia RG, Zarruk JG, Guzman JC, Barrera C, Pinzon A, Trillos E, et al. Sex differences in cardiac autonomic function of depressed young adults. Biol Psychol. 2012;90: 179–185. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Basso L, Boecking B, Neff P, Brueggemann P, Cederroth CR, Rose M, et al. Sex Differences in Comorbidity Combinations in the Swedish Population. Biomol. 2022;12: 949. doi: 10.3390/biom12070949 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Chatoui H, Abbaoui A, Hiba OE, Draoui A, Gamrani H. Neurobehavioral and neurophysiological effects of prolonged osmotic stress in rats: A focus on anxiety state and pain perception. J Chem Neuroanat. 2020;106: 101789. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2020.101789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Corrêa M dos S, Vaz B dos S, Grisanti GDV, Paiva JPQ de, Tiba PA, Fornari RV. Relationship between footshock intensity, post-training corticosterone release and contextual fear memory specificity over time. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2019;110: 104447. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Ma L, Xu Y, Wang G, Li R. What do we know about sex differences in depression: A review of animal models and potential mechanisms. Prog Neuro-psychopharmacology Biological Psychiatry. 2019;89: 48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.08.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Prendergast BJ, Onishi KG, Zucker I. Female mice liberated for inclusion in neuroscience and biomedical research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;40: 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Kalsbeek A, Spek R van der, Lei J, Endert E, Buijs RM, Fliers E. Circadian rhythms in the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2012;349: 20–29. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2011.06.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Chaaya N, Jacques A, Belmer A, Beecher K, Ali SA, Chehrehasa F, et al. Contextual Fear Conditioning Alter Microglia Number and Morphology in the Rat Dorsal Hippocampus. Front Cell Neurosci. 2019;13: 214. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2019.00214 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Enomoto S, Kato TA. Involvement of microglia in disturbed fear memory regulation: Possible microglial contribution to the pathophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurochem Int. 2021;142: 104921. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2020.104921 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Hu L, Zhu S, Peng X, Li K, Peng W, Zhong Y, et al. High Salt Elicits Brain Inflammation and Cognitive Dysfunction, Accompanied by Alternations in the Gut Microbiota and Decreased SCFA Production. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2020;77: 629–640. doi: 10.3233/JAD-200035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Zhang T, Wang D, Li X, Jiang Y, Wang C, Zhang Y, et al. Excess salt intake promotes M1 microglia polarization via a p38/MAPK/AR-dependent pathway after cerebral ischemia in mice. Int Immunopharmacol. 2020;81: 106176. doi: 10.1016/j.intimp.2019.106176 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Chan SY, Capitão L, Probert F, Klinge C, Hoeckner S, Harmer CJ, et al. A single administration of the antibiotic, minocycline, reduces fear processing and improves implicit learning in healthy volunteers: analysis of the serum metabolome. Transl Psychiat. 2020;10: 148. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0818-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Thiago P Fernandes

Transfer Alert

This paper was transferred from another journal. As a result, its full editorial history (including decision letters, peer reviews and author responses) may not be present.

7 Mar 2023

PONE-D-22-32310Generalization of contextual fear is sex-specifically affected by high salt intakePLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Gilman,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 21 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Thiago Fernandes, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure: 

Funding for this work was provided by Kent State University, and the Applied Psychology Center in the Department of Psychological Sciences at Kent State University.  

  

Please state what role the funders took in the study.  If the funders had no role, please state: "The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript." 

If this statement is not correct you must amend it as needed. 

Please include this amended Role of Funder statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3. Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript: 

Funding for this work was provided by Kent State University, and the Applied Psychology Center in the Department of Psychological Sciences at Kent State University.  

However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form. 

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows: 

Funding for this work was provided by Kent State University, and the Applied Psychology Center in the Department of Psychological Sciences at Kent State University.  

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. Please amend your list of authors on the manuscript to ensure that each author is linked to an affiliation. Authors’ affiliations should reflect the institution where the work was done (if authors moved subsequently, you can also list the new affiliation stating “current affiliation:….” as necessary).

5. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information. 

Additional Editor Comments:

Please respond to each point AND highlight all changes.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This is an interesting study. The following issues should be addressed.

1. Fig. 2. Adjust the Y axis to reflect the low freezing levels seen. Also, it would be better to show these data as bar graphs so that group differences can be appreciated.

2. Only 5 seconds post the shock are analyzed. It seems better to analyze the largest ISI possible.

3. Females and males respond different to singly housing. Therefore, it is conceivable that in experiment 3 sex-dependent effects of singly housing might have contributed to the sex-dependent effects seen. This should be acknowledged.

4. It seems better to move figure panels not showing any significance from the main figure to supplementary figures. Some figure, like figure 6, could be a main or supplementary table as well. Fig. 4 is the main figure and receives less attention now.

5. There seems more of a sex difference in the training context in Experiments 1 and 2 in Fig. 4 than in Experiment 3. One wonder whether this might have contributed to what is seen in Experiment 3. In females, the pattern in the neutral context seems definitely similar in Experiments 2 and 3.

6. The salt effect in females in the neutral context seems subtle and not different between the two salt diet groups. The salt effect in males in the neutral context seems more pronounced.

7. It would be better to show the real cort data so that others can compare with other designs.

8. It would be better to remove p values from the figures, especially if they do not reach significance.

9. The text in the results section is written very stats heavy. While all correct, this makes it harder to read for those less versed in statistical interactions. The authors are encouraged to rephrase some of the text to make this manuscript more readable. The tables with the stats data are good and allow focusing less on stats result in the text.

Reviewer #2: This manuscript evaluates that possibility that a forced, high-salt diet affects contextual fear acquisition, recall and generalization in male and female C57BL/6J mice. I think there is some important data here, and it should be seen. Most of my concerns are with how the data, statistics, etc. are presented. Given that this is a straight-forward experiment, it would benefit to attempt to streamline what is being presented to the reader. I found myself paying too much attention to small details the were being presented/suggested rather than being able to focus on the main message (or two) as I read through the Results and Discussion presentation.

Specific comments:

Introduction: Lines 68-70 – I think it would be useful to provide some specific detail on how hippocampus/amygdala are affected by high-salt.

Consider providing a clear “N per group/sex” in the Methods.

Describe in more detail why 5x shock was used. 1 or two is sufficient to engender contextual fear.

Page 8, Line 158 – Much more thorough justification needs to be provided for why the authors are selectively presenting/analyzing data from minutes 2-6 out of the 10-minute test. Without this, it appears that the data has been “cherry picked” to show some effects.

Figure 2 – It is confusing to refer to a “post-shock period” when these experiments did not employ shock. Also, I find using the sex-symbols as data points to be distracting. Could you more simply use blue/red squares and circles?

Consider placing the tables in a data supplement. There are 10 of them, several with multiple parts, and they are essentially all ANOVA tables.

Page 26, Line 526 – The overall statistical analyses appear to be rigorous. The notion of a “trend” presented in the Discussion detracts from this.

Page 27, Line 548 – The above is also true for the notion of a “bimodal” distribution, which would be very difficult to show with that number of data points.

Page 28, Line 579 – Same for “non-significant trends”

How do the authors know that 0.8mA is not painful?

While I think the authors are likely correct about the lack of social simulation and light-phase testing in their experiment, bringing this up distracts from the overall message regarding the data at hand. For example, it begs the question why reverse-phase lighting was not used in the first place?

Page 28, Line 566 – Sentence beginning “Nonetheless…”. I would like the authors to expand more upon this. From a biological standpoint, I’m left hanging and wondering “how would this be possible?”. Perhaps the authors can draw on other work for examples outside of hypertension/anxiety.

Do the authors think their findings would generalize to more classic mouse tests of “anxiety”, such as EPM or open field?

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Thiago P Fernandes

11 May 2023

Generalization of contextual fear is sex-specifically affected by high salt intake

PONE-D-22-32310R1

Dear Dr. Gilman,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Thiago Fernandes, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors did a fine job addressing the raised concerns. No additional edits are requested from this reviewer.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Thiago P Fernandes

16 May 2023

PONE-D-22-32310R1

Generalization of contextual fear is sex-specifically affected by high salt intake

Dear Dr. Gilman:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Thiago P. Fernandes

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on context fear ‘training’ for control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S2 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on context fear testing of control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S3 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on context fear training for context fear conditioned mice of both sexes in Experiment 1.

    (PDF)

    S4 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on context fear training for context fear conditioned mice of both sexes in Experiment 2.

    (PDF)

    S5 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on context fear training for context fear conditioned mice of both sexes in Experiment 3.

    (PDF)

    S6 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on full 10 min time course of context fear testing for control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S7 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on context fear expression (min 2–6) for context fear conditioned mice across Experiment.

    (PDF)

    S8 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on full 10 min time course of context fear testing for mice of both sexes in Experiment 1.

    (PDF)

    S9 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on full 10 min time course of context fear testing for mice of both sexes in Experiment 2.

    (PDF)

    S10 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on full 10 min time course of context fear testing for mice of both sexes in Experiment 3.

    (PDF)

    S11 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on serum osmolality in control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S12 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on serum osmolality in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S13 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on log-transformed serum corticosterone levels in control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S14 Table. Three-way ANOVAs on log-transformed serum corticosterone in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S15 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average water consumption per day for context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S16 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average food consumption per day for context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S17 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly body weight change for context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S18 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on twice weekly body weight measurements of context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S19 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average water consumption per day for control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S20 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average food consumption per day for control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S21 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly body weight changes for control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S22 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on twice weekly body weights of control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S23 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average NaCl consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S24 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average NaCl consumed as a percentage of body weight in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S25 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly ratio of water to NaCl consumed in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S26 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average kcal consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S27 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly kcal consumed as a percentage of body weight in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S28 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average NaCl consumed per day by control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S29 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly NaCl consumed as a percentage of body weight by control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S30 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly water to NaCl ratio consumed by control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S31 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average kcal consumed per day by control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S32 Table. Three-way repeated measures ANOVAs on weekly average kcal consumed as a percentage of body weight by control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S33 Table. Details of confirmed outliers (> 5 standard deviations (SD) ± average of data set sans suspected outlier) within respective data sets.

    Outliers are marked with red font.

    (PDF)

    S1 Fig. Fear behavior in no shock control mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S2 Fig. Fear behavior in no shock control mice across Experiments with shortened Y axes.

    (PDF)

    S3 Fig. Time course of context fear expression testing in no shock groups.

    (PDF)

    S4 Fig. Time course of context fear expression testing in shock groups.

    (PDF)

    S5 Fig. Serum osmolality in no shock control mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S6 Fig. Log-transformed serum corticosterone levels of no shock control mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S7 Fig. Raw (pre-transformed) serum corticosterone levels of no shock control mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S8 Fig. Raw (pre-transformed) serum corticosterone levels in fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S9 Fig. Average water consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S10 Fig. Average food consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S11 Fig. Weekly body weight change in context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S12 Fig. Body weight of context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S13 Fig. Average water and food consumed per day by control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S14 Fig. Body weights and body weight changes in control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S15 Fig. Average NaCl consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S16 Fig. NaCl consumed as a percentage of body weight by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S17 Fig. Water to NaCl consumed ratio for context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S18 Fig. Average kcal consumed per day by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S19 Fig. Consumed kcal as a percentage of body weight by context fear conditioned mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S20 Fig. Average NaCl consumed per day and NaCl consumed as a percentage of body weight by control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S21 Fig. Ratio of water to NaCl consumed by control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S22 Fig. Average kcal consumed per day and kcal consumed as a percentage of body weight by control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S23 Fig. Correlations between context fear expression and log-transformed serum corticosterone levels across contexts and Experiments.

    (PDF)

    S24 Fig. Correlations between context fear expression and log-transformed serum corticosterone levels in control no shock mice across Experiments.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    The data are accessible at the following DOI: 10.17605/OSF.IO/E5K2M.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES